2021
DOI: 10.1093/brain/awab400
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Aphasia localization: was Pierre Marie right?

Abstract: Language and its associated disorders have puzzled humanity since the dawn of civilization. The first descriptions of aphasia go back to classical antiquity. The Egyptians and Babylonians believed speech was a divine gift to mortals, and their descriptions of aphasia attributed these events to their Gods' anger and disfavor. The Edwin Smith Surgical Papyrus and the Hippocratic Corpus report several aphasia cases, relating this phenomenology to apoplexy, epilepsy, and other illnesses.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is also important to interpret our findings within a growing set of literatures suggesting that the LIFC clearly plays multiple roles in both language-specific and domain-general functions (Fedorenko & Blank, 2020;Novick et al, 2005Novick et al, , 2010Matchin, 2018;Friederici, 2020;Grodzinsky & Santi, 2008;Hagoort, 2013;Matchin & Hickok, 2020;Tremblay & Dick, 2016;Zaccarella et al, 2017). Moreover, while LIFC clearly plays a role as a hub of the speech production system, the neural architecture supporting speech production is complex, widespread, and context-dependent (Hickok et al, 2022;Bulut, 2022;Coutinho et al, 2021). Indeed, whereas the anterior portion of LIFC (pars triangularis or Brodmann's Area 45) is often associated with semantic processing (Schell et al, 2017;Friederici, 2011Friederici, , 2012Hagoort, 2013), the posterior portion of LIFC (pars opercularis or Brodmann's Area 44) is involved in syntactic processing (Hagoort, 2013;Ullman, 2016;Friederici, 2011Friederici, , 2012 and domain-general cognitive control processes (Novick et al, 2010;Fedorenko & Blank, 2020), in addition to being strongly linked to multiple features of high-level speech production, including phonological retrieval (Matchin, 2018), lexical selection (Conner et al, 2019), and syllabic sequencing (Rong et al, 2018;Hickok et al, 2022).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…It is also important to interpret our findings within a growing set of literatures suggesting that the LIFC clearly plays multiple roles in both language-specific and domain-general functions (Fedorenko & Blank, 2020;Novick et al, 2005Novick et al, , 2010Matchin, 2018;Friederici, 2020;Grodzinsky & Santi, 2008;Hagoort, 2013;Matchin & Hickok, 2020;Tremblay & Dick, 2016;Zaccarella et al, 2017). Moreover, while LIFC clearly plays a role as a hub of the speech production system, the neural architecture supporting speech production is complex, widespread, and context-dependent (Hickok et al, 2022;Bulut, 2022;Coutinho et al, 2021). Indeed, whereas the anterior portion of LIFC (pars triangularis or Brodmann's Area 45) is often associated with semantic processing (Schell et al, 2017;Friederici, 2011Friederici, , 2012Hagoort, 2013), the posterior portion of LIFC (pars opercularis or Brodmann's Area 44) is involved in syntactic processing (Hagoort, 2013;Ullman, 2016;Friederici, 2011Friederici, , 2012 and domain-general cognitive control processes (Novick et al, 2010;Fedorenko & Blank, 2020), in addition to being strongly linked to multiple features of high-level speech production, including phonological retrieval (Matchin, 2018), lexical selection (Conner et al, 2019), and syllabic sequencing (Rong et al, 2018;Hickok et al, 2022).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…It is also important to interpret our findings within a growing set of literatures suggesting that the LIFC clearly plays multiple roles in both language‐specific and domain‐general functions (Fedorenko & Blank, 2020; Friederici, 2020; Grodzinsky & Santi, 2008; Hagoort, 2013; Matchin, 2018; Matchin & Hickok, 2020; Novick et al, 2005, 2010; Tremblay & Dick, 2016; Zaccarella et al, 2017). Moreover, while LIFC clearly plays a role as a hub of the speech production system, the neural architecture supporting speech production is complex, widespread, and context‐dependent (Bulut, 2022; Coutinho et al, 2021). Indeed, whereas the anterior portion of LIFC (pars triangularis or Brodmann's Area 45) is often associated with semantic processing (Friederici, 2011, 2012; Hagoort, 2013; Schell et al, 2017), the posterior portion of LIFC (pars opercularis or Brodmann's Area 44) is involved in syntactic processing (Friederici, 2011, 2012; Hagoort, 2013; Ullman, 2016) and domain‐general cognitive control processes (Fedorenko & Blank, 2020; Novick et al, 2010), in addition to being strongly linked to multiple features of high‐level speech production, including phonological retrieval (Matchin, 2018), lexical selection (Conner et al, 2019), and syllabic sequencing (Rong et al, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%