2020
DOI: 10.1177/2167702620929636
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Anxiety Modulates Preference for Immediate Rewards Among Trait-Impulsive Individuals: A Hierarchical Bayesian Analysis

Abstract: Trait impulsivity—defined by strong preference for immediate over delayed rewards and difficulties inhibiting prepotent behaviors—is observed in all externalizing disorders, including substance-use disorders. Many laboratory tasks have been developed to identify decision-making mechanisms and correlates of impulsive behavior, but convergence between task measures and self-reports of impulsivity are consistently low. Long-standing theories of personality and decision-making predict that neurally mediated indivi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
8
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 118 publications
1
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These problems, however, may largely reflect the use of person‐level summary statistics that (a) do not retain trial‐to‐trial variability within persons and (b) do not examine individual differences in cognitive processes thought to underlie performance on the task (Haines, Kvam, et al., 2020; Rouder & Haaf, 2019). Recent work on this topic has highlighted the importance of allowing for heterogeneity in experimental effects in data analyses (i.e., relative to simpler ANOVA‐style analysis; Bolger et al., 2019) and the value of computational cognitive models that provide testable predictions about the processes that putatively generate performance on tasks (Haines, Beauchaine, et al., 2020).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These problems, however, may largely reflect the use of person‐level summary statistics that (a) do not retain trial‐to‐trial variability within persons and (b) do not examine individual differences in cognitive processes thought to underlie performance on the task (Haines, Kvam, et al., 2020; Rouder & Haaf, 2019). Recent work on this topic has highlighted the importance of allowing for heterogeneity in experimental effects in data analyses (i.e., relative to simpler ANOVA‐style analysis; Bolger et al., 2019) and the value of computational cognitive models that provide testable predictions about the processes that putatively generate performance on tasks (Haines, Beauchaine, et al., 2020).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Joint modelling allows maximum use of participant-level data, whilst retaining information about uncertainty or precision of each kind of measurement (Turner et al, 2017;Haines et al, 2020a;Haines, 2021;Hopkins et al, 2021).…”
Section: Combining Self-report and Task Behaviour Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For the joint model, individual estimates for trait amotivation (θ A ) and/or trait negative cognition (θ N ; constructed as above), were allowed to influence the effect of intervention on time 2 parameter estimates (ϕ IN T ) found to show evidence of heterogeneous individual responses via the inclusion of additional β weight parameters (β IN T ; see Haines et al 2020a;Hopkins et al 2021) for previous examples of this approach). These β weights can interpreted similarly as in a standard regression model, with the group-level intervention effect (ϕ IN T ) now representing the intercept (see below).…”
Section: Combining Self-report and Task Behaviour Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Brooks et al, 2011) most popularly used for Bayesian model fitting 2 (such as JAGS: Plummer, 2003; and Stan: Carpenter et al, 2017) and software that likewise automates Bayesian model specification (for linear models: Bürkner, 2017; and for select cognitive models: Ahn et al, 2017). These developments have made Bayesian cognitive modeling accessible to psychologists across subfields, including cognitive psychologists (e.g., Donkin et al, 2016; Navarro et al, 2016; Westfall & Lee, 2021), cognitive neuroscientists (e.g., Frank et al, 2015; Nunez et al, 2019; Peters & D’Esposito, 2020), clinical psychologists (e.g., Haines et al, 2020; Brown et al, 2021; Lasagna et al, 2022), and social psychologists (e.g., Pleskac et al, 2018; Golubickis et al, 2018; Schaper et al, 2019).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While using Bayesian methods for cognitive modeling had long been the province of mathematical psychologists, as it required comfort with mathematical statistics and statistical programming (Gilks et al, 1995;Gelman et al, 2013; for an example, see Rouder & Lu, 2005), this has changed with the maturation of software that automates the Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) methods (S. Brooks et al, 2011) most popularly used for Bayesian model fitting 2 (such as JAGS : Plummer, 2003;and Stan: Carpenter et al, 2017) and software that likewise automates Bayesian model specification (for linear models : Bürkner, 2017; and for select cognitive models: Ahn et al, 2017). These developments have made Bayesian cognitive modeling accessible to psychologists across subfields, including cognitive psychologists (e.g., Donkin et al, 2016;Navarro et al, 2016;Westfall & Lee, 2021), cognitive neuroscientists (e.g., Frank et al, 2015;Nunez et al, 2019;Peters & D'Esposito, 2020), clinical psychologists (e.g., Haines et al, 2020;Brown et al, 2021;Lasagna et al, 2022), and social psychologists (e.g., Pleskac et al, 2018;Golubickis et al, 2018;Schaper et al, 2019).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%