2001
DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.2.166
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Anxiety and cognitive inhibition.

Abstract: In 3 experiments, the authors investigated whether anxiety proneness is associated with impaired inhibitory processing. Participants made speeded decisions requiring inhibition of threatening or neutral meanings of ambiguous words, which were inappropriate in their current context. In Experiment 1 there were no differences found in inhibitory processing associated with anxiety. However, in Experiment 2, when the capacity for controlled processing was reduced by imposition of a mental load, anxious individuals … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

6
50
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 64 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
(48 reference statements)
6
50
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One explanation for this is that individuals, regardless of anxious state, found it more difficult to disengage from the salient fearful faces [Derryberry and Reed, 2002;Fox et al, 2002]. Cognitive control may have been recruited to ignore fearful faces and thus diverted resources away from attention performance on the subsequent task [Wood et al, 2001]. This is consistent with resource allocation models which hypothesize that facilitation of emotional processing can divert attentional resources to the detriment of other cognitive processes [Compton, 2003;Easterbrook, 1959;Meinhardt and Pekrun, 2003].…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 73%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…One explanation for this is that individuals, regardless of anxious state, found it more difficult to disengage from the salient fearful faces [Derryberry and Reed, 2002;Fox et al, 2002]. Cognitive control may have been recruited to ignore fearful faces and thus diverted resources away from attention performance on the subsequent task [Wood et al, 2001]. This is consistent with resource allocation models which hypothesize that facilitation of emotional processing can divert attentional resources to the detriment of other cognitive processes [Compton, 2003;Easterbrook, 1959;Meinhardt and Pekrun, 2003].…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 73%
“…First, anxious individuals, although often performing a primary attention task as well as low anxious individuals, show attention deficits when there is a secondary or dual task-relevant demand [Wood et al, 2001]. This suggests that depletion of general cognitive resources has detrimental effects on attention, but does not speak to whether threat-related attention interference effects are due to increased processing of the affective stimuli Easterbrook, 1959;Ellis and Ashbrook, 1989;Hanoch and Vitouch, 2004;Kliegel et al, 2003;Mogg et al, 2000;Rokke et al, 2002;Schupp et al, 2003;Vuilleumier and Schwartz, 2001].…”
Section: Threat-related Attentional Biases and Anxietymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Threat-sensitivity is more likely to interfere with attention when it exceeds an optimal level: for example, elevated anxiety has been shown to increase the negative impact of threat-related emotional stimuli on executive attention (Jazbec et al, 2005;Mathews and Mackintosh, 1998;Wood et al, 2001) such as conflict interference tasks (Fenske and Eastwood, 2003;Williams et al, 1996).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%