1992
DOI: 10.1016/0163-1047(92)90351-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Anterograde and retrograde enhancement of 24-h memory by glucose in elderly humans

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

14
53
0
1

Year Published

1996
1996
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 104 publications
(68 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
14
53
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This further supports the idea that materials of any valence or arousal value could be enhanced by posttraining memory modulation techniques. This result is consistent with several previous studies (Colrain et al, 1992;Manning et al, 1992;Nielson & Bryant, 2005;Nielson & Jensen, 1994;Nielson et al, 1996Nielson et al, , 2005 and contrasts with contentions that materials must have inherent affective or arousal quality for memory modulation to occur (e.g., Libkuman, Nichols-Whitehead, Griffith, & Thomas, 1999).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This further supports the idea that materials of any valence or arousal value could be enhanced by posttraining memory modulation techniques. This result is consistent with several previous studies (Colrain et al, 1992;Manning et al, 1992;Nielson & Bryant, 2005;Nielson & Jensen, 1994;Nielson et al, 1996Nielson et al, , 2005 and contrasts with contentions that materials must have inherent affective or arousal quality for memory modulation to occur (e.g., Libkuman, Nichols-Whitehead, Griffith, & Thomas, 1999).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…As such, it is impossible to decipher in these studies whether the effect is on attention, encoding, consolidation or some combination of these phases. However, several recent studies have demonstrated memory modulatory effects in human participants by comparable mechanisms of action as have been shown in rodent studies using various posttraining treatments including norepinephrine (Southwick et al, 2002), glucose (Manning, Parsons, & Gold, 1992), nicotine (Colrain, Mangan, Pellett, & Bates, 1992), and non-invasive treatments such as muscle tension (Nielson & Jensen, 1994;Nielson, Radtke, & Jensen, 1996), stress (Cahill, Gorski, & Le, 2003) and negative and positive emotional arousal (Nielson & Bryant, 2005;Nielson, Yee, & Erickson, 2005). Importantly, no human study has yet examined the time-dependency of these effects; no study has varied the delay of the intervention after learning.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 72%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Livingston (1967) proposed such an idea, that hormone response to stress that occurs after learning can modulate memory for any recently acquired information. Our findings are consistent with this proposition, as were a number of other studies using an arousing treatment after learning of non-emotive stimuli (Colrain et al, 1992;Manning et al, 1992;Nielson & Jensen, 1994;Nielson et al, 1996). However, the present findings contrast with a recent few recent studies that suggest that post-learning arousal may only be effective to modulate inherently arousing stimuli (Cahill, Gorski, & Le, 2003) or that some amount of arousal or novelty at encoding is necessary for post-learning arousal treatments to modulate memory Okuda, Roozendaal, & McGaugh, 2004).…”
supporting
confidence: 93%
“…Nicotine (Colrain, Mangan, Pellett, & Bates, 1992), glucose (Manning, Parsons, & Gold, 1992), and muscle tension (Nielson & Jensen, 1994;Nielson, Radtke, & Jensen, 1996) have been shown to enhance delayed retrieval of non-arousing memory materials when given after learning. For example, Nielson and Jensen (1994) showed that induction of muscle tension shortly after exposure to target words embedded in paragraphs increased heart rate and enhanced delayed recall and recognition of the words, except in participants who were taking β-blockers to control hypertension.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%