2015
DOI: 10.1515/tlr-2015-0011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Animating the narrow syntax

Abstract: Abstract:In this paper we demonstrate that there are two related, but distinct types of animacy in the Plains Algonquian language, Blackfoot -morphological animacy (m-animacy) and high animacy (H-animacy). We argue that the two types of animacy are constructed in different ways: M-animacy is a head feature that determines noun class and plays a role in syntactic agreement operations, whereas H-animacy is a selectable feature of arguments. The two kinds of animacy also have different distributions: Only languag… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
16
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
(29 reference statements)
1
16
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This evidence comes from the crosslinguistic behavior and distribution of determiners and classifiers (Haspelmath , Lyons , Cowper & Hall , Ionin , Simpson, Soh & Nomoto , Cowper & Hall ); the features of indefinite pronouns (Fodor & Sag , von Heusinger ); nonscopal specific interpretations of indefinites (Fodor & Sag , von Heusinger ); and patterns of acquisition of determiners in second‐language acquisition (Ionin, Ko & Wexler , Ionin, Zubizarreta & Philippov ). There is also ample evidence for features like [ human ] and [ animate ], for example, from verbal marking in Algonquian languages (Piggott , Wiltschko & Ritter , among others); verbal prefixes in Abkhaz (Hewitt , cited by Mithun ); nominal marking in Selayarese (Finer ); and impersonal pronouns crosslinguistically (Sigurðsson & Egerland , Fenger ).…”
Section: Proposalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This evidence comes from the crosslinguistic behavior and distribution of determiners and classifiers (Haspelmath , Lyons , Cowper & Hall , Ionin , Simpson, Soh & Nomoto , Cowper & Hall ); the features of indefinite pronouns (Fodor & Sag , von Heusinger ); nonscopal specific interpretations of indefinites (Fodor & Sag , von Heusinger ); and patterns of acquisition of determiners in second‐language acquisition (Ionin, Ko & Wexler , Ionin, Zubizarreta & Philippov ). There is also ample evidence for features like [ human ] and [ animate ], for example, from verbal marking in Algonquian languages (Piggott , Wiltschko & Ritter , among others); verbal prefixes in Abkhaz (Hewitt , cited by Mithun ); nominal marking in Selayarese (Finer ); and impersonal pronouns crosslinguistically (Sigurðsson & Egerland , Fenger ).…”
Section: Proposalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In particular, a sentience restriction in the language has been recognized to be a defining property that distinguishes nominals that carry a grammatical function -subject or primary object -from those which do not, such as adjuncts (e.g., Frantz 2009; Kim 2015a; Ritter and Rosen 2010; Wiltschko and Ritter 2015): subjects and primary objects must be sentient, unlike adjuncts. In recent studies on Blackfoot, the sentience restriction has been proposed to be a part of narrow syntax, represented as syntactic agreement (Kim 2017b;Wiltschko and Ritter 2015). 15 In light of these studies, the sentience restriction is an important criterion in Blackfoot to determine whether a given oblique nominal is a primary object or an adjunct.…”
Section: Sentience Restrictionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(cf. Wiltschko & Ritter 2015 for discussion). 4 Note that for morphologically intransitive verbs, the designated argument whose animacy is specified is the subject, but for morphologically transitive verbs, the designated argument is the primary object.…”
Section: Two Two-way Contrasts: the Role Of The Verbal Predicatementioning
confidence: 99%