2008
DOI: 10.3758/lb.36.2.104
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Analysis of a trial-spacing effect with relatively long intertrial intervals

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
33
0
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
4
33
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In the present rat magazine-entry paradigm, the trial appears to be far more important in both conditioning and extinction than the time-accumulation view recognizes. For example, related research suggests that the effects of even temporal variables such as CS duration and trial spacing may be more consistent with SOP theory (e.g., Wagner, 1981; Wagner & Brandon, 1983), a trial-based model that explicitly recognizes a role for time, than with the time-accumulation view (Bouton & Sunsay, 2003; Moody et al, 2006; Sunsay & Bouton, 2008; Sunsay et al, 2004). Extant time-accumulation models are thus not ready to replace trial-based models.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the present rat magazine-entry paradigm, the trial appears to be far more important in both conditioning and extinction than the time-accumulation view recognizes. For example, related research suggests that the effects of even temporal variables such as CS duration and trial spacing may be more consistent with SOP theory (e.g., Wagner, 1981; Wagner & Brandon, 1983), a trial-based model that explicitly recognizes a role for time, than with the time-accumulation view (Bouton & Sunsay, 2003; Moody et al, 2006; Sunsay & Bouton, 2008; Sunsay et al, 2004). Extant time-accumulation models are thus not ready to replace trial-based models.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The benefits of this effect have been attributed to efficient acquisition of memory (35) and superior retention (36). Studies using an appetitive conditioning task and taste aversion learning in rats demonstrated that longer trial spacing facilitated the acquisition of memory (37,38). In contrast, an odor avoidance task performed in Drosophila, sensitization of the gill withdrawal reflex in Aplysia, and place navigation task performed in rats have shown facilitation of memory retention by spacing (10,16,18).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is widely known that trials that are spaced over a period of time result in stronger learning than temporally massed trials; this is true for appetitive (food) conditioning (e.g., Sunsay & Bouton, 2008; Sunsay et al, 2004) as well as other kinds of conditioning (e.g., Barela, 1999; Domjan, 1980). At a theoretical level, spaced trials may enhance learning by increasing the novelty or surprisingness of the cue and the significant outcome (Sunsay & Bouton, 2008); they might also improve memory strength if it is partially forgotten between learning episodes (Bjork & Bjork, 1992) or the learning in extinction of inhibition if error correction (like responding itself) spontaneously recovers over time (e.g., Leung & Westbrook, 2008). Although there is ample theory to suggest that spaced extinction trials would improve extinction learning, trial spacing during extinction training has produced inconsistent results in animal experiments (e.g., compare results of Cain, Blouin, & Barad, 2003; Moody, Sunsay, & Bouton, 2006b; Urcelay, Wheeler, & Miller, 2009).…”
Section: How To Improve Extinction Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%