2005
DOI: 10.1080/09638230500270842
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

An experimental investigation of the impact of biological versus psychological explanations of the cause of “mental illness”

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
35
2

Year Published

2008
2008
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 55 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
4
35
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Consistent with attribution theory, information about biological causes decreases the tendency to blame people with mental illness for their condition (Boysen, 2011;Boysen & Vogel, 2006;Crisafulli, Von Holle, & Bulik, 2008;Deacon & Baird, 2008;Lincoln et al, 2008). However, biological information also leads people to doubt the ability of individuals to recover from mental illness (Deacon & Baird, 2008;Lam & Salkovskis, 2007;Lam, Salkovskis, & Warwick, 2005;Lincoln et al, 2008;Phelan, 2005). Although the findings are less consistent, biological information has also led to increased perceptions of people with mental illness as dangerous (Lam & Salkovskis, 2007;Lam et al, 2005;Read & Law, 1999), unpredictable (Read & Law, 1999), and requiring coercive actions (Phelan, 2005).…”
Section: Etiological Beliefs and Mental Illness Stigmamentioning
confidence: 71%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Consistent with attribution theory, information about biological causes decreases the tendency to blame people with mental illness for their condition (Boysen, 2011;Boysen & Vogel, 2006;Crisafulli, Von Holle, & Bulik, 2008;Deacon & Baird, 2008;Lincoln et al, 2008). However, biological information also leads people to doubt the ability of individuals to recover from mental illness (Deacon & Baird, 2008;Lam & Salkovskis, 2007;Lam, Salkovskis, & Warwick, 2005;Lincoln et al, 2008;Phelan, 2005). Although the findings are less consistent, biological information has also led to increased perceptions of people with mental illness as dangerous (Lam & Salkovskis, 2007;Lam et al, 2005;Read & Law, 1999), unpredictable (Read & Law, 1999), and requiring coercive actions (Phelan, 2005).…”
Section: Etiological Beliefs and Mental Illness Stigmamentioning
confidence: 71%
“…However, biological information also leads people to doubt the ability of individuals to recover from mental illness (Deacon & Baird, 2008;Lam & Salkovskis, 2007;Lam, Salkovskis, & Warwick, 2005;Lincoln et al, 2008;Phelan, 2005). Although the findings are less consistent, biological information has also led to increased perceptions of people with mental illness as dangerous (Lam & Salkovskis, 2007;Lam et al, 2005;Read & Law, 1999), unpredictable (Read & Law, 1999), and requiring coercive actions (Phelan, 2005). These results suggest that past antistigma efforts by groups such as the National Alliance on Mental Illness could have actually increased some forms of stigma by portraying mental illnesses as brain diseases.…”
Section: Etiological Beliefs and Mental Illness Stigmamentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In their analysis of ADHD discourses, Bröer and Heerings [24] classified public and personal discourses in terms of the categories 'sociology', 'spirituality', 'psychology', 'neurobiology' and 'advantages'. Previous analyses of public understandings of addiction have classified discourses into 'biogenetic', 'brain disease', 'biological' or 'psychological' [22,29]. While we expected some overlap between academic and lay terminologies and classifications, we did not assume that members of the public would necessarily use technical vocabularies or classifications in the same way as experts.…”
Section: Coding and Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some leading researchers have argued that increased public acceptance of a neurobiological conception of addiction will have important ethical implications for reducing stigma and thereby improving treatment outcomes for addicted individuals (Lebowitz and Ahn, 2012), beliefs that are used to justify neuroscience education campaigns (Vrecko, 2010a). Some empirical studies support the possibility of realising the destigmatising aspirations of addiction neuroscience proponents (Furnham and Rees, 1988;Mehta and Farina, 1997), whereas more recent studies do not (Lam et al, 2005;Angermeyer et al, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%