2006
DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.523
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

An emotion-induced attentional blink elicited by aversively conditioned stimuli.

Abstract: The current study examines whether aversively conditioned stimuli can modulate attention to such a degree that they impair the perception of subsequently presented nonemotional targets. In the initial phase of this study, participants viewed 3 categories of photographs, 1 of which was paired with an aversive noise. Following conditioning, participants searched for a target embedded within a series of 17 rapidly presented images on each trial. Critically, a conditioned or unconditioned item from the initial pha… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
132
4

Year Published

2008
2008
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 135 publications
(143 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
(27 reference statements)
7
132
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Further, found little evidence of this sparing effect for Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) images, suggesting that the effect may be isolated to alphanumerical stimuli. Finally, such an explanation is at odds with evidence showing that salient distractors in fact induce a deficit for subsequent targets, similar to that observed in the AB (Most, Chun, Widders, & Zald, 2005;Smith et al, 2006). …”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Further, found little evidence of this sparing effect for Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) images, suggesting that the effect may be isolated to alphanumerical stimuli. Finally, such an explanation is at odds with evidence showing that salient distractors in fact induce a deficit for subsequent targets, similar to that observed in the AB (Most, Chun, Widders, & Zald, 2005;Smith et al, 2006). …”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…For example, targets with a degree of learnt salience, such as one's own name (Shapiro, Caldwell, & Sorenson, 1997), a famous face (Jackson & Raymond, 2006), or an emotionally salient word (Anderson, 2005;Anderson & Phelps, 2001) show a reduced impairment relative to more neutral stimuli. Similarly, when a stimulus that has been previously paired with an unpleasant outcome is used as a distractor in an RSVP task, it can automatically generate an impairment for subsequent targets, similar to that observed in the AB (Smith, Most, Newsome, & Zald, 2006). Thus, there is some evidence to suggest that stimuli with either familiarised or conditioned importance gain a competitive advantage under conditions of constrained attention.…”
Section: Timementioning
confidence: 83%
“…The majority of investigations reviewed indicated that the interaction of the conditions or groups of interest with lag was due to differences between conditions or groups at shorter, but not at longer, lags (e.g., Akyürek, Hommel, & Jolicoeur, 2007;Arend, Johnston, & Shapiro, 2007;Colzato, Slagter, Spapé, & Hommel, 2008;Rokke et al, 2002;Shapiro et al 1997b;Smith, Most, Newsome, & Zald, 2006;Stein et al, 2009). This pattern provides a clear case for concluding that the AB effect, the posttarget impairment confined to a critical period, was modulated (see Fig.…”
Section: Capturing Modulations Of the Ab Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After three PS+ and three PS2 trials, there were two CS+ trials paired with the shock. These "booster" trials were included to reduce habituation (e.g., Smith et al 2006;Lim and Pessoa 2008), and data from these trials were not included in any analyses. Three additional PS+ and PS2 trials followed the "booster" trials.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%