1968
DOI: 10.1163/156853968x00270
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

An Analysis of the Phonoresponse of Males of the True Katydid, Pterophylla Camellifolia (Fabricius) (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae)

Abstract: Abstract1. Males of the true katydid, Pterophylla camellifolia F., produce three kinds of acoustical signals termed calling (which can be subdivided into solo calling and alternating calling), aggressive, and disturbance sounds. Alternating calling and aggressive sounds are specialized phonoresponses consisting of regular, rhythmic alternation of chirps by adjacent males at rates slower than solo calling. The arhythmic disturbance sounds are elicited by handling. 2. The nature of alternation was investigated b… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
33
0

Year Published

1972
1972
2002
2002

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 61 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
1
33
0
Order By: Relevance
“…4), thus raising the suspicion that it is an artifact of some sort. Studies of other acoustical insects overwhelmingly support the existence of linear functions relating temperature to most song parameters that repeat over time, as best exemplified by and documented for rates of wing-stroking, chirping, and "rolling" in crickeis (Alexander 1956, Walker 1962, Dumortier 1963, Prestwich and Walker 1981) and a few katydids and homopterans (Dumortier 1963, Shaw 1968, Whitesell and Walker 1978, Booij 1982. Similarly, most of the temperature data reported here for Ch.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 93%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…4), thus raising the suspicion that it is an artifact of some sort. Studies of other acoustical insects overwhelmingly support the existence of linear functions relating temperature to most song parameters that repeat over time, as best exemplified by and documented for rates of wing-stroking, chirping, and "rolling" in crickeis (Alexander 1956, Walker 1962, Dumortier 1963, Prestwich and Walker 1981) and a few katydids and homopterans (Dumortier 1963, Shaw 1968, Whitesell and Walker 1978, Booij 1982. Similarly, most of the temperature data reported here for Ch.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…If such signals are to be effective as reproductive isolating mechanisms, however, they must remain unambiguous to recipients over the wide range of temperature typically experienced by lacewings in the field. Abundant documentation exists of gross alteration in chirp rate, wing-stroke frequency, pulse or chirp duration, or call notes by temperature changes in many taxonomically disparate insect groups (Brooks 1882, Hayward 1901, Alexander 1956, Walker 1962, Dumortier 1963, Shaw 1968, Booij 1982 (Henry 1980a(Henry , 1982b Fig. 3A), while terminal portions remain within a much narrower range of values over equivalent temperature extremes (slope 0.960x).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Intermediate phenomena and combinations of synchrony and alternation exist in some species. For example, the participants in a synchronous chorus may only partially overlap their songs (46,131,138), and alternating choruses may be punctuated by occasional occurrences of synchrony (2,78,130). As above, consistent leaders, individuals whose rhythm is slightly advanced relative to their neighbors, may exist among synchronizers (97,124).…”
Section: Annual Reviews Wwwannualreviewsorg/aronlinementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Pholidoptera griseoaptera : Jones, 1966). Leaders and followers (where leaders are defined as males that typically initiate calling after a period of silence) can again be distinguished in some species, such as Pterophylla camellifolia (Shaw, 1968).…”
Section: Chorusesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This inhibitory-resetting mechanism affects one call period only, with the natural rhythm continuing thereafter. Playback experiments have shown that rhythms and signal interactions in several species fit the assumptions of their model, for example in Pholidoptera griseoaptera (Jones, 1966), Pterophylla camellifolia (Shaw, 1968), Oecanthus fultoni (Walker, 1969), and Ligurotettix planum . Greenfield et al's simulations show that, given female choice for leading calls, the basic resetting mechanism is evolutionarily stable, as long as it includes a 'relativity adjustment' for the velocity of signal transmission and selective attention towards a subset of signalling neighbours.…”
Section: Chorusesmentioning
confidence: 99%