2004
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-004-1833-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Amplitude and direction errors in kinesthetic pointing

Abstract: We investigated the accuracy with which, in the absence of vision, one can reach again a 2D target location that had been previously identified by a guided movement. A robotic arm guided the participant's hand to a target (locating motion) and away from it (homing motion). Then, the participant pointed freely toward the remembered target position. Two experiments manipulated separately the kinematics of the locating and homing motions. Some robot motions followed a straight path with the bell-shaped velocity p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
15
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
6
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…First, the most conspicuous aspect of the systematic error, the bias of most oblique orientations toward the closest diagonal, has been observed in various visual tasks such as reproducing the location of a point in a circle (Huttenlocher et al, 1991;Huttenlocher et al, 2004) and perceiving oriented lines (de Graaf et al, 1994;Lennie, 1971;Smyrmis et al, 2007;Zlatkova, 1993) or random dot patterns (Yakimoff, Lansky, Mitrani, & Radil, 1989). A similar bias has also been observed in pointing tasks toward remembered targets presented in a circular arrangement in the kinesthetic (Baud-Bovy & Viviani, 2004) and visual modalities (e.g., Gordon, Ghilardi, & Ghez, 1995;Gourtzelidis et al, 2001;Smyrmis et al, 2007). Second, our results are also in line with the widespread observation that the reproduction of the vertical and horizontal orientations is more accurate than the reproduction of oblique orientations, which has also been cited in many different visual tasks (e.g., Appelle, 1972;Essock, 1980;Keene, 1963;Westheimer, 2003;Zlatkova, 1993).…”
Section: External Validity Of the Extended Ca Modelsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…First, the most conspicuous aspect of the systematic error, the bias of most oblique orientations toward the closest diagonal, has been observed in various visual tasks such as reproducing the location of a point in a circle (Huttenlocher et al, 1991;Huttenlocher et al, 2004) and perceiving oriented lines (de Graaf et al, 1994;Lennie, 1971;Smyrmis et al, 2007;Zlatkova, 1993) or random dot patterns (Yakimoff, Lansky, Mitrani, & Radil, 1989). A similar bias has also been observed in pointing tasks toward remembered targets presented in a circular arrangement in the kinesthetic (Baud-Bovy & Viviani, 2004) and visual modalities (e.g., Gordon, Ghilardi, & Ghez, 1995;Gourtzelidis et al, 2001;Smyrmis et al, 2007). Second, our results are also in line with the widespread observation that the reproduction of the vertical and horizontal orientations is more accurate than the reproduction of oblique orientations, which has also been cited in many different visual tasks (e.g., Appelle, 1972;Essock, 1980;Keene, 1963;Westheimer, 2003;Zlatkova, 1993).…”
Section: External Validity Of the Extended Ca Modelsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…In particular, it does not explain the tendency to overestimate orientations between 0°a nd 90°, and underestimate them between 90°and 180°. Additional factors might be at play such as the viewing angle in the visual modality (Hermens & Gielen, 2003; see also Dick & Hochstein, 1989;Keene, 1963) or a bias in the perceived orientation of the arm segments in the haptic modality (e.g., Baud-Bovy & Viviani, 2004).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Oblique effects for response variability have been observed in visual (Bauer et al, 1979;Essock, 1980;Heeley et al, 1997;Gentaz et al, 2001), motor (Baud-Bovy and Viviani, 2004;Smyrnis et al, 2007), and haptic (Lechelt et al, 1976;Baud-Bovy and Gentaz, 2006) perceptual tasks. The role of gravity in defining the vertical axes for oblique effects has been studied by tilting the head and body with respect to gravity (Buchanan-Smith and Heeley, 1993;Lipshits and McIntyre, 1999;Luyat et al, 2001;Luyat and Gentaz, 2002) and by performing these experiments in weightlessness McIntyre, 1999, 2007;Lipshits et al, 2005).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Accuracy measurements in visual or motor-matching tasks typically reveal biases in responses away from the vertical and horizontal axes (tilt contrast) in both visual (Reese, 1953;Dick and Hochstein, 1989;Yakimoff et al, 1989) and motor (Gordon et al, 1994;Baud-Bovy and Viviani, 2004;Smyrnis et al, 2007) tasks. These effects have variously been ascribed to (1) an exaggeration of deviations away from the meridians (Gibson, 1966;Howard, 1982), (2) attraction of responses toward the diagonal (Yakimoff et al, 1989), or (3) artifacts related to an implicit projection of stimuli in depth (Dick and Hochstein, 1989).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The oblique effect is also present at the cognitive level (Olson & Hildyard 1977) and is termed "oblique effect of class 2" (Essock, 1980). The same phenomenon has been reported to occur in the kinesthetic perceptual system (Baud-Bovy & Viviani, 2004) and for the motor control (Smyrnis et al, 2007). According to Gentaz (Gentaz, 2005) the vertical axis is privileged because it gives the gravitation direction and the horizontal axis is also privileged because it corresponds to the visual horizon.…”
Section: Common Cross-modalities Reference Frame For Orientationsmentioning
confidence: 72%