1986
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.1986.45-283
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Allocation of Complex, Sequential Operants on Multiple and Concurrent Schedules of Reinforcement

Abstract: Pigeons could produce food by pecking exactly four times on each of two keys, in any order. In the first experiment, these response sequences were reinforced on a series of multiple schedules of variableinterval reinforcement. In the second experiment, these response sequences were reinforced on a series of concurrent schedules of reinforcement. In both experiments, highly stereotyped response sequences developed. If these response sequences were treated as individual responses, the resulting data conformed to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
15
0

Year Published

1987
1987
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
(34 reference statements)
0
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…She may also have developed stereotyped response sequences that operated as functional behavioral units (Schwartz, 1986) and that were resistant to change (Nevin, Mandell, & Atak, 1983). She tended to respond exclusively or near-exclusively to the VI 30-s schedule when the delay to reinforcer access was short, and exclusively to the leaner schedule (producing immediate reinforcer access) when the delay to reinforcer access on the richer schedule was longest.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…She may also have developed stereotyped response sequences that operated as functional behavioral units (Schwartz, 1986) and that were resistant to change (Nevin, Mandell, & Atak, 1983). She tended to respond exclusively or near-exclusively to the VI 30-s schedule when the delay to reinforcer access was short, and exclusively to the leaner schedule (producing immediate reinforcer access) when the delay to reinforcer access on the richer schedule was longest.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bouts may also have behavioral-unit like properties (Brackney et al, 2011;Shull et al, 2001). If this is the case, bouts of different lengths may belong to different response classes, and the frequency of bouts of different lengths may have some correspondence to the frequency at which they are reinforced (Bach a-M endez, Reid, & Mendoza-Soylovna, 2007;Schwartz, 1986;Shimp, 1982). Unfortunately, the differential reinforcement of specific bout lengths cannot be explicitly tested due to the probabilistic nature of starts and ends of individual bouts (for extended discussions of bout detection methods, see Brackney et al, 2011;Cheung et al, 2012;Shull et al, 2001Shull et al, , 2002.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In his search for the fundamental units of behavior, Skinner (1935) defined a response unit as a functional class of events subject to control by reinforcing stimuli. A lever press could become a behavioral unit, but so could interresponse times (IRTs) (e.g., Morse, 1966;Platt, 1973;Zeiler, 1977) and integrated sequences of simple responses (e.g., Fetterman & Stubbs, 1982;Grayson & Wasserman, 1979;Hawkes & Shimp, 1975;Reid, Chadwick, Dunham, & Miller, 2001;Schwartz, 1981Schwartz, , 1982Schwartz, , 1986Shimp, 1976Shimp, , 1979. Zeiler (1977) identified three different kinds of response units that can be distinguished: formal, conditionable, and theoretical units.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%