2003
DOI: 10.2190/6939-6w88-p2xx-guqw
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Age Identification in the Framework of Successful Aging. A Study of Older Finnish People

Abstract: A person-oriented approach was used in a study of age identification among community-dwelling older people. The study was based on 8-year follow-up data; 843 persons aged 65-84 were involved in the first phase of the study, and 426 persons aged 73-92, in the second phase. Loosely, on the basis of the distinction between successful, usual, and pathological aging (Rowe & Kahn, 1987, 1997), participants were grouped according to their self-ratings of cognitive and physical functioning as "Positive," "Negative," a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

2
48
0
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(51 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
2
48
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…whether the respondent felt fearful, lonely, or useless in daily life (see Zeng et al 2013). A successful ager should have none of these depressive symptoms (see Uotinen et al 2003). 4.…”
Section: Successful Ageingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…whether the respondent felt fearful, lonely, or useless in daily life (see Zeng et al 2013). A successful ager should have none of these depressive symptoms (see Uotinen et al 2003). 4.…”
Section: Successful Ageingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Montepare and Lachman (1989) have summarized this view: "Theorists in the aging field have suggested that the tendency of aging adults to maintain younger subjective age identities is a form of defensive denial by which they can dissociate themselves from the stigma attached to growing old" (p. 73). Following this age-denial view, a youthful subjective age is an indicator of successful aging (e.g., Uotinen, Suutama, & Ruoppila, 2003), which is consistent with studies showing that subjective age is a better predictor of psychological and physical functioning in old age than is chronological age (e.g., Barak & Stern, 1986;Markides & Boldt, 1983;Peters, 1971; but see Knoll, Rieckmann, & Scholz, 2004). Under this age-denial view, there is no reason to suspect a discrepancy between subjective and chronological age in childhood and early adulthood, but the discrepancy should accelerate with older ages.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A large amount of research supports the validity of using subjective age as a means of understanding the behaviors and attitudes of adults across their lifespan (Birren & Cunningham, 1985;George, Mutran, & Pennypacker, 1980;Henderson et al, 1995;Kastenbaum, Derbin, Sabatini, & Arrt, 1972;Neugarten & Hagestad, 1976;Van Auken, Barry, & Anderson, 1993). Subjective age has been shown to be a predictor of many behavioral and psychological variables, including life satisfaction, self-esteem, perceived health, fear of aging, boredom levels in adults, maturity, body satisfaction, and leisure activities (Barnes-Farrell & Piotraski, 1989;Gana, Alaphillippe, & Bailly, 2004;Galombos, Turner, & Tilton-Weaver, 2005;Montepare, 1996;Montepare & Lachman, 1989;Palmore, 1981;Uotinen, Suutama, & Ruoppila, 2003;Westerhof & Barrett, 2005). Barak and Stern (1986) noted that women define their subjective age as younger than their chronological age as they grow older.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%