2006
DOI: 10.3758/bf03194001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Age and sex differences in children’s spatial search strategies

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
16
1

Year Published

2009
2009
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
(18 reference statements)
1
16
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Children's searches were also related to gender, such that on average girls (M = 8.13, SE = 1.62) searched closer to the center of the landmark arrays than did boys (M = 13.20, SE = 1.61), F(1, 66) = 4.95, P = .03, η 2 = .07. This was in contrast to the findings of Spetch and Parent [14] showing an advantage for boys in a similar search task. Future research should further investigate the role of gender in children's search strategies.…”
Section: 13contrasting
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Children's searches were also related to gender, such that on average girls (M = 8.13, SE = 1.62) searched closer to the center of the landmark arrays than did boys (M = 13.20, SE = 1.61), F(1, 66) = 4.95, P = .03, η 2 = .07. This was in contrast to the findings of Spetch and Parent [14] showing an advantage for boys in a similar search task. Future research should further investigate the role of gender in children's search strategies.…”
Section: 13contrasting
confidence: 99%
“…Our findings regarding experimenter-provided spatial language cues provide a possible explanation for why previous studies have shown differing results regarding children's ability to use multiple landmarks [2,[13][14][15][16]. Whereas some studies show that children can use the middle relation to find a hidden object by four or five years of age [2,15], other studies show that children at these ages do not always use a middle search strategy when other methods of solving the task are possible [16].…”
Section: Spatial Language Cuesmentioning
confidence: 66%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…tasks that require the solution of two utiknowns via vector algebra. Together with extant evidence that the behavior of pigeons (Spetch, Rust, Kamil, & Jones, 2003;Sturz & Katz, 2009), nutcrackers (Kamil & Jones, 1997,2000, rats (Btown & Terrinoni, 1996), gerbUs (Collet, Cartwright, & Smith, 1986), dogs (Fiset, 2009), human children (Spetch & Parent, 2006), and adults (Spetch, Cheng, & MacDonald, 1996;Spetch et al, 1997) are capable of leaming complex spatial relationships (for reviews, see Brown, 2006;Cheng & Spetch, 1998;Spetch & Kelly, 2006), the current results add to growing evidence that complex spatial relationships are teamed during navigation and that the leaming of such complex spatial relationships can be understood via vector-based models of navigation. Regardless of whether or not the unique demands of navigation fashioned these actual vectorbased representational and computational cognitive abilities to cope with the numerous and often complex spatial relationships that exist in nature, vector-based models of navigation appear to be viable approaches to understanding complex spatial behavior.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…At 4 years of age, children are successful in differentiating between two identical objects by their relation to a nearby landmark (being close or far), a platform's edge (next to it or in the middle of the platform), and a platform edge's length (at the shorter edge or at the longer edge) (Vasilyeva, 2002). However, at the same age, they fail when they need to encode a hiding container in relation to two other containers in the array-one identical to the hiding container and one unique, which could potentially be used as a landmark (Lee, Shusterman, & Spelke, 2006), but children can learn through repeated trials that a reward is always hidden in the middle of two landmarks (Simms & Gentner, 2008;Spetch & Parent, 2006;Uttal, Sandstrom, & Newcombe, 2006). Hence, it appears that 4-year-olds readily encode the location of a hiding place in relation to a single landmark but still have great difficulty in encoding location in terms of two relations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%