2015
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12292
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Affective matching of odors and facial expressions in infants: shifting patterns between 3 and 7 months

Abstract: Recognition of emotional facial expressions is a crucial skill for adaptive behavior. Past research suggests that at 5 to 7 months of age, infants look longer to an unfamiliar dynamic angry/happy face which emotionally matches a vocal expression. This suggests that they can match stimulations of distinct modalities on their emotional content. In the present study, olfaction-vision matching abilities were assessed across different age groups (3, 5 and 7 months) using dynamic expressive faces (happy vs. disguste… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

2
20
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
2
20
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Multi‐sensory integration processing follows a ‘motor hierarchy’ (for a review, see Burr & Gori, ). For example, integration that relies on orienting reflexes (e.g., reflexive eye and hand movements) develops quite early (Bahrick & Lickliter, ; Godard et al., ; Kuhl & Meltzoff, ; Sann & Streri, ), while other forms of integration that rely on non‐reflexive, explicit motor responses continue to develop throughout childhood (Gori et al., ; Nardini et al., ; Nardini et al., ). To test the developmental trajectory of explicit, non‐reflexive visuo‐olfactory affective matching, we measured a goal‐directed motor response (e.g., pointing).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Multi‐sensory integration processing follows a ‘motor hierarchy’ (for a review, see Burr & Gori, ). For example, integration that relies on orienting reflexes (e.g., reflexive eye and hand movements) develops quite early (Bahrick & Lickliter, ; Godard et al., ; Kuhl & Meltzoff, ; Sann & Streri, ), while other forms of integration that rely on non‐reflexive, explicit motor responses continue to develop throughout childhood (Gori et al., ; Nardini et al., ; Nardini et al., ). To test the developmental trajectory of explicit, non‐reflexive visuo‐olfactory affective matching, we measured a goal‐directed motor response (e.g., pointing).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the process of recognizing emotional facial expressions relies not only on visual exteroceptive information, but also on non-visual interoceptive signals arising from the observer’s own internal states [39,40]. Interestingly, food-mood interactions have been widely investigated and the effects of different food substances on emotional states have been consistently demonstrated [23], providing clear evidence that taste and odours are strong and reliable elicitors of emotional reactions and the related facial expressions [22,41,42]. For example, Macht and Dettmer demonstrated that participants’ positive mood increased after eating chocolate or an apple, with a stronger and longer-lasting effect after chocolate intake [24].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent studies on 3-month old infants showed that after the babies inhaled a pleasant odour—such as strawberry—they spent more time looking towards a happy face than a disgusted one [22]. According to Godard and colleagues, the odour might have elicited a pleasant feeling, eventually activating facial muscles normally recruited when displaying that positive emotional state.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations