2012
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-012-0255-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Adding the goal to learn strengthens learning in an unintentional learning task

Abstract: Previous research has demonstrated that contingency learning can take place in the absence of the intention to learn. For instance, in the color-word contingency learning task, each distracting word is presented most often in a given target color (e.g., "month" in red and "plate" in green), and less often in the other colors. Participants respond more quickly and accurately when the word is presented in the expected rather than an unexpected color, even though there is no reason why they would have the intenti… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
33
4

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

4
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
(27 reference statements)
5
33
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Interestingly, participants generally have very low levels of contingency awareness in this task (Schmidt & De Houwer, 2012a, 2012b, 2012c, 2012d, similar to other tasks (e.g., Destrebecqz & Cleeremans, 2001;Lewicki, 1985;McKelvie, 1987;Miller, 1987).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Interestingly, participants generally have very low levels of contingency awareness in this task (Schmidt & De Houwer, 2012a, 2012b, 2012c, 2012d, similar to other tasks (e.g., Destrebecqz & Cleeremans, 2001;Lewicki, 1985;McKelvie, 1987;Miller, 1987).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Using the highly simplistic colour-word contingency learning paradigm, Schmidt and De Houwer (2012a) gave half the participants the goal to learn contingencies (i.e., they were not told the actual pairings, but asked to discover them). These participants showed a larger contingency effect than controls.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One example of this is the colour-word contingency learning paradigm of Schmidt and colleagues (Schmidt & Besner, 2008;Schmidt, Crump, Cheesman, & Besner, 2007;Schmidt & De Houwer, 2012a, 2012b, 2012cSchmidt, De Houwer, & Besner, 2010). In this colour identification paradigm, each of a set of neutral words is presented most often in a certain print colour (e.g., "month" in red, "plate" in green, etc.).…”
Section: Learning Awareness and Instruction: Subjective Contingencymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Said differently, words end up being predictive of their congruent response (e.g., BLUE is predictive of a blue response because it is presented in blue QUESTIONING CONFLICT ADAPTATION 24 most often). Even in non-conflict tasks, any such predictive word-response relationships will be rapidly learned by participants (e.g., Schmidt & De Houwer, 2012a, 2012b, 2012cSchmidt, De Houwer, & Besner, 2010). With such contingency biases in the task, congruent trials are high contingency (predictive of the correct response) and incongruent trials are low contingency (not predictive).…”
Section: Contingency Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%