2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.028
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Adaptive vocal behavior drives perception by echolocation in bats

Abstract: Echolocation operates through adaptive sensorimotor systems that collectively enable the bat to localize and track sonar objects as it flies. The features of sonar signals used by a bat to probe its surroundings determine the information available to its acoustic imaging system. In turn, the bat’s perception of a complex scene guides its active adjustments in the features of subsequent sonar vocalizations. Here, we propose that the bat’s active vocal-motor behaviors play directly into its representation of a d… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

2
66
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 67 publications
(68 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
2
66
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Each scene is dynamic: the animal and its prey are moving through space, which produces changes in the features of echo returns. The task is further complicated in a cluttered environment where each sonar emission results in a cascade of echoes arriving from different locations, which the bat must organize into a coherent representation Moss et al, 2011;Simmons et al, 1988). Acoustic cues, such as interaural time, intensity and spectral differences, provide information about the direction of a sonar object (Shimozawa et al, 1974;Simmons et al, 1983), whereas echo arrival time provides information about its distance (Ewer, 1945;Simmons, 1973).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Each scene is dynamic: the animal and its prey are moving through space, which produces changes in the features of echo returns. The task is further complicated in a cluttered environment where each sonar emission results in a cascade of echoes arriving from different locations, which the bat must organize into a coherent representation Moss et al, 2011;Simmons et al, 1988). Acoustic cues, such as interaural time, intensity and spectral differences, provide information about the direction of a sonar object (Shimozawa et al, 1974;Simmons et al, 1983), whereas echo arrival time provides information about its distance (Ewer, 1945;Simmons, 1973).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Dynamic changes to the acoustic field of view (Jakobsen et al, 2013;Wisniewska et al, 2012) may help echolocating animals inspect their surroundings or lock on to specific targets, shaping the perception of their surroundings via changes in the acoustic gaze (Moss, 2010;Moss et al, 2011). Here, we show that wild Atlantic spotted dolphins seem to increase their vertical biosonar beam width by 50% over a fourfold decrease in range.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 71%
“…The beam width and intensity of emitted signals depend on their spectral and temporal properties and on the acoustic behaviour of the echolocating animal (Moss and Surlykke, 2001). There is increasing evidence that bats and toothed whales exhibit significant control over their biosonar (Jakobsen and Surlykke, 2010;Johnson et al, 2008;Moore et al, 2008;Wisniewska et al, 2012) and it is likely that they actively control the perception of their surroundings through changes in biosonar signals and biosonar field of view (Moss et al, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bat echolocation is a superior model for studying active perception, because ongoing dynamic adaptation of echolocation signals provides a direct window to scene analysis in a naturally behaving animal (32)(33)(34), allowing general inferences for active motor response to perceptual sensory feedback. The acoustic and behavioral median reaction times of around 80-90 ms (Figs.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%