1965
DOI: 10.2466/pr0.1965.17.3.767
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Acquisition and Transfer of Simultaneous Oddity

Abstract: The matching-to-sample experimental procedure was altered by reinforcing the selection of the non-matching comparison hue rather than the matching stimulus. Six birds were trained with red, green, and blue alternatives and a simultaneous presentation of stimuli in which the sample was present at the same time as the choice stimuli. The acquisition functions began well above the chance level but displayed a very slow improvement thereafter, which was different from that shown under matching conditions. Transfer… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

8
52
4
4

Year Published

1971
1971
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 65 publications
(68 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
8
52
4
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Because the words theta and upsilon deviated from the familiar word pi, the subjects apparently ''responded away from'' or excluded the previously positive ⌸ and instead selected the previously negative ⌰ or ⌼. Although the finding supported an earlier study by Vincent-Smith, Bricker, and Bricker (1974), it contradicted the commonly held belief that subjects learn the positive relation between a given sample or discriminative stimulus and the correct alternative but not the negative relation between the same sample and the incorrect alternative (see, e.g., Berryman, Cumming, Cohen, & Johnson, 1965;Carter & Werner, 1978;Skinner, 1950). Dixon's (1977) study demonstrated that subjects performing relational learning tasks may be capable of learning not only what is correct but also what is incorrect.…”
contrasting
confidence: 53%
“…Because the words theta and upsilon deviated from the familiar word pi, the subjects apparently ''responded away from'' or excluded the previously positive ⌸ and instead selected the previously negative ⌰ or ⌼. Although the finding supported an earlier study by Vincent-Smith, Bricker, and Bricker (1974), it contradicted the commonly held belief that subjects learn the positive relation between a given sample or discriminative stimulus and the correct alternative but not the negative relation between the same sample and the incorrect alternative (see, e.g., Berryman, Cumming, Cohen, & Johnson, 1965;Carter & Werner, 1978;Skinner, 1950). Dixon's (1977) study demonstrated that subjects performing relational learning tasks may be capable of learning not only what is correct but also what is incorrect.…”
contrasting
confidence: 53%
“…But if these pigeons ignored sample location, only two rules were needed to support accurate performance (e.g., "choose vertical after red" and "choose horizontal after green") in comparison with four for the location-relevant group ("choose vertical after left-red," "choose horizontal after left-green," "choose horizontal after right-red," and "choose vertical after right-green"). So why didn't fewer rules produce faster learning as in other conditional discriminations (Berryman, Cumming, Cohen, & Johnson, 1965)? One answer is that acquisition rate depended on when sample location came to be ignored in training.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In oddity, each ST designates one CO-and two (or more) cOt stimuli. Evidence from transfer tests following extensive original training provides evidence that, for both simple matching and simple oddity, the pigeon selects the COt but does not avoid the CO-when in the presence of a particular ST Berryman, Cumming, Cohen, & Johnson, 1965). Thatis, the pigeon is controlled by the S+ rules (ST implies CO+) but not by the S-rules (ST implies CO-) in both the matching and the oddity cases.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%