1922
DOI: 10.1364/josa.6.000343
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Theory of Intermittent Vision

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
32
0

Year Published

1929
1929
1994
1994

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 118 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
0
32
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We make the axiomatic assumption that temporal integration and segregation arise from interactions not between the physical stimuli, but between the corresponding visual responses. The transfer from physical stimuli to visual responses is predicated on the assumption that, in its peripheral stages, the visual system acts as a linear temporal fIlter (Ives, 1922;Kelly, 1961). On this assumption, any given visual response can be represented as the output of a linear fIlter with input provided by the physical stimulus.…”
Section: The Temporal Correlation Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We make the axiomatic assumption that temporal integration and segregation arise from interactions not between the physical stimuli, but between the corresponding visual responses. The transfer from physical stimuli to visual responses is predicated on the assumption that, in its peripheral stages, the visual system acts as a linear temporal fIlter (Ives, 1922;Kelly, 1961). On this assumption, any given visual response can be represented as the output of a linear fIlter with input provided by the physical stimulus.…”
Section: The Temporal Correlation Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Either implicit or explicit in the various sampling models are the assumptions that (1) there is some unidimensional sensory representation of the stimulus that materializes full-blown either at the instant of stimulus onset or shortly thereafter; (2) that in the absence of a mask, the representation decays, eventually to zero, with decay beginning at stimulus offset; and (3) that the magnitude of this sensory representation is constant during stimulus presence and independent of stimulus duration. Although this viewpoint is simple and mathematically convenient, it is at odds with a great deal of sensory literature which implies that (1) the sensory representation rises gradually, not abruptly, following stimulus onset; (2) the peak strength of the representation-and hence the onset of decay-occurs sometime after stimulus offset; and (3) the sensory representation's peak magnitude is positively related to stimulus duration (e.g., de Lange, 1952;Ives, 1922;Kelly, 1961;see Cornsweet, 1971, pp. 384-418, andWatson, 1986, for thorough discussions of this literature).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ever since the introduction of sine-wave-modulated flicker to the study of temporal factors in vision (DeLange, 1952;Ives, 1922) and of the sine-wave grating to studies of spatial vision (e.g., Robson, 1966;Schade, 1956), the use of linear systems theory to characterize human performance in visual tasks has been widespread. An image-processing system can be useful in this kind of research, both as a tool for simulating models of human performance and for the generation of psychophysical stimuli.…”
Section: Sine-waves and Linear Systems Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%