2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A survey of neuropsychologists’ beliefs and practices with respect to the assessment of effort

Abstract: The current study investigated neuropsychologists' beliefs and practices with respect to assessing effort and malingering by surveying a sample of NAN professional members and fellows (n=712). The results from 188 (26.4%) returned surveys indicated that 57% of respondents frequently included measures of effort when conducting a neuropsychological evaluation. While a majority of respondents (52%) rarely or never provide a warning that effort indicators will be administered, 27% of respondents often or always pr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
93
0
4

Year Published

2008
2008
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 227 publications
(99 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
2
93
0
4
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition to the lack of consensus regarding optimal cut-off raw scores, studies are quite variable in terms of the base rates of malingering in their samples; as examples, it was 100% in Larrabee (1998) and ranged from 25 to 50% in the table for positive predictive power of the FBS presented in Greiffenstein et al (2004), with their suggestion that 50% is common in worker's compensation settings. These high rates are not unusual in FBS studies, yet are well above the 1 to 20% base rate for malingering reported by Sharland and Gfeller (2007) in their survey of practitioners. How well these high FBS base rates for malingering apply across most settings that use the MMPI-2 is questionable, at best, including those involving psychological injury.…”
Section: Evaluating Fbs Responding: Raw Score Cut-offs and Normsmentioning
confidence: 59%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In addition to the lack of consensus regarding optimal cut-off raw scores, studies are quite variable in terms of the base rates of malingering in their samples; as examples, it was 100% in Larrabee (1998) and ranged from 25 to 50% in the table for positive predictive power of the FBS presented in Greiffenstein et al (2004), with their suggestion that 50% is common in worker's compensation settings. These high rates are not unusual in FBS studies, yet are well above the 1 to 20% base rate for malingering reported by Sharland and Gfeller (2007) in their survey of practitioners. How well these high FBS base rates for malingering apply across most settings that use the MMPI-2 is questionable, at best, including those involving psychological injury.…”
Section: Evaluating Fbs Responding: Raw Score Cut-offs and Normsmentioning
confidence: 59%
“…A survey by Sharland and Gfeller (2007) suggested that the FBS was among the top five most frequently used measures of effort or response bias used by doctoral level members of the National Academy of Neuropsychology (NAN), although their small sample (n=188) and low response rate of 26% precludes generalizability. Furthermore, their conclusions that 75% of those responding used the FBS are offset by the data presented in their tables (i.e., only 43.2% actually reported using the FBS "often" or "always").…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Whilst such practice is recommended (Iverson, 2006) and North American surveys suggest that between one-quarter (Slick et al, 2004) and one-third (Sharland & Gfeller, 2007) of clinicians already ''often or always'' use warnings prior to testing, the effect of warnings on genuine clients must be considered. The present study did not address this issue; however, in previous work we have shown that warnings do not alter the performance of participants who are trying to perform at the best of their ability, at least when memory tests are used (Sullivan, Keane, & Deffenti, 2002).…”
Section: Limitations and Conclusionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In terms of basic test usage, researchers have reported on the most commonly used instruments (Butler, Retzlaff, & Vanderploeg, 1991;Camara, Nathan, & Puente, 2000;Rabin et al, 2005), as well as those utilized for specific purposes such as forensic evaluations (Lees-Haley et al, 1996) or within the broad cognitive areas (i.e., memory, attention, executive functioning; Rabin et al, 2005). Recent surveys by Slick et al (2004) and Sharland and Gfeller (2007) have focused on measures used to assess effort, malingering, and/or response bias. Thus, a development in neuropsychological survey work appears to be the identification of instruments used within specific cognitive and/or functional domains.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%