1976
DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.61.2.234
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A statistical power analysis of applied psychological research.

Abstract: The average statistical power exhibited in the 1974 volume of the Journal oj Applied Psychology was studied. One hundred twenty-one articles were poweranalytically examined using three effect-size estimates, standardized (.05) alpha level, and assuming nondirectional nulls. The mean power figures for small, medium, and large population effect-size estimates were .25, .66, and .84, respectively. Interdisciplinary comparisons involving abnormal-social psychology, education, and communication indicate that applie… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
45
1
1

Year Published

1978
1978
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 105 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
1
45
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The belief is that the consequences of a false positive (Type I error) claim are more serious than those of a false negative (Type II error) claim. As a result, Type I errors are usually focused on more frequently and guarded against more stringently by researchers (Baroudi & Orlikowski, 1989;Brewer, 1972;Chase & Chase, 1976;Cohen, 1977;Cowles & Davis, 1982;Greenwald, 1993;Mazen, Graf, et al, 1987;Myers & Melcher, 1969;Mone et al, 1996;Nickerson, 2000;Rossi, 1990;Sedlmeier & Gigerenzer, 1989). However, it is important to note that several authors (e.g., Mazen, Graf, et al, 1987;Sauley & Bedeian, 1989) advocate distributing the risk between Type I and Type II errors because the risks/consequences associated with committing a Type II error may be extremely costly.…”
Section: Significance Levelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The belief is that the consequences of a false positive (Type I error) claim are more serious than those of a false negative (Type II error) claim. As a result, Type I errors are usually focused on more frequently and guarded against more stringently by researchers (Baroudi & Orlikowski, 1989;Brewer, 1972;Chase & Chase, 1976;Cohen, 1977;Cowles & Davis, 1982;Greenwald, 1993;Mazen, Graf, et al, 1987;Myers & Melcher, 1969;Mone et al, 1996;Nickerson, 2000;Rossi, 1990;Sedlmeier & Gigerenzer, 1989). However, it is important to note that several authors (e.g., Mazen, Graf, et al, 1987;Sauley & Bedeian, 1989) advocate distributing the risk between Type I and Type II errors because the risks/consequences associated with committing a Type II error may be extremely costly.…”
Section: Significance Levelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…His findings of minimal power values of statistical tests in published research has been replicated by Brewer (1972) in educational research and by Chase and Chase (1976) in applied psychology.…”
mentioning
confidence: 80%
“…However, if they cannot tolerate the adjusted levels of alpha, power or both, that is, if alpha is too high or power too low to be justifiable from a research point of view, then they should consider postponing the study until they can get a larger sample. Cohen (1962), Brewer (1972), Jones and Brewer (1972), Penick and Brewer (1972), Brewer and Owen (1973), Katzer and Sodt (1973), and Chase and Chase (1976), respectively, the researchers found a dismal state of affairs with regard to the power of the tests conducted, as indicated in Table 2. Table 2: Results of studies on the power of hypotheses tests…”
Section: Implications Of Not Meeting Sample Size Requirementsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Journal of Applied Psychology Chase and Chase (1976) . 25 .67 .86 known, as one does not have the population scores at hand.…”
Section: Implications Of Not Meeting Sample Size Requirementsmentioning
confidence: 99%