The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
2019
DOI: 10.1007/s00163-018-00305-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A state-transition model of team conceptual design activity

Abstract:  Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research.  You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain  You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
33
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 91 publications
0
33
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Contextually-modulated behavioral transitions during creative tasks have been shown to positively correlate with creative performance using think-aloud protocols in ecologically complex creative design tasks [4] and in cognitive control tasks such as the color-word Stroop task [3]. For example, during complex creative design tasks, experienced designers have been found to continually move between different subgoals as they iteratively and progressively discover, define, and seek to address emerging design issues or opportunities [5][6][7], and the frequency of such within-task transitions increases both with greater expertise, and leads to higher quality design outcomes [4]. Within experimental psychology, questions relating to cognitive flexibility have been extensively examined using experimenter-cued task-switching paradigms (e.g., number-letter tasks) [1,8] and set-shifting tasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task [9] and related tasks [10].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Contextually-modulated behavioral transitions during creative tasks have been shown to positively correlate with creative performance using think-aloud protocols in ecologically complex creative design tasks [4] and in cognitive control tasks such as the color-word Stroop task [3]. For example, during complex creative design tasks, experienced designers have been found to continually move between different subgoals as they iteratively and progressively discover, define, and seek to address emerging design issues or opportunities [5][6][7], and the frequency of such within-task transitions increases both with greater expertise, and leads to higher quality design outcomes [4]. Within experimental psychology, questions relating to cognitive flexibility have been extensively examined using experimenter-cued task-switching paradigms (e.g., number-letter tasks) [1,8] and set-shifting tasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task [9] and related tasks [10].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, the assumption that analysis is primarily related to the problem space exploration, as it is often instructed by the prescriptive design literature, should be revisited in the context of team designing. Namely, this assumption goes beyond simple counting of instances where teams perform problem analysis versus instances where they perform solution analysis (this has already been addressed in one of the previous studies; please consult [43]). It instead correlates analysis (no matter if performed in the problem or solution space) with problem space exploration, meaning that analysis-intensive activity is also likely to be more intensive in terms of exploring the problem space (to the degree it can be observed in the design team's dialogue).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, there are also many design contexts in which group work is a key element and where responsibility for idea production and idea development is effectively shared (e.g. see Kohn & Smith, 2011;Martinec, Škec, Horvat, & Štorga, 2019). This is not addressed in the accounts we offer here and also not emphasised in many of the case studies conducted previously (but see Dogan & Nersessian, 2010;Cross, 2001).…”
Section: Limitations and Future Workmentioning
confidence: 99%