2002
DOI: 10.1007/s12110-002-1017-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A proximate perspective on reciprocal altruism

Abstract: The study of reciprocal altruism, or the exchange of goods and services between individuals, requires attention to both evolutionary explanations and proximate mechanisms. Evolutionary explanations have been debated at length, but far less is known about the proximate mechanisms of reciprocity. Our own research has focused on the immediate causes and contingencies underlying services such as food sharing, grooming, and cooperation in brown capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees. Employing both observational and expe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

6
212
0
11

Year Published

2006
2006
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 293 publications
(229 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
(23 reference statements)
6
212
0
11
Order By: Relevance
“…Virtually all organisms exhibit social behaviors that are flexible or conditional on the behaviors of others, from bacteria (e.g., quorum sensing; Miller and Bassler 2001) to insects (e.g., reproductive strategies depending on social context; WestEberhard 1987), birds (e.g., responses of parents to each other; Wright and Cuthill 1989), primates (e.g., reciprocal altruism; Brosnan and de Waal 2002), and obviously, humans. Not surprisingly, a large theoretical literature focuses on the evolutionary consequences of particular kinds of flexible (or conditional) behavior (e.g., Axelrod and Hamilton 1981;McNamara et al 1999; Lehmann and Keller 2006;Akçay et al 2009;Boyd et al 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Virtually all organisms exhibit social behaviors that are flexible or conditional on the behaviors of others, from bacteria (e.g., quorum sensing; Miller and Bassler 2001) to insects (e.g., reproductive strategies depending on social context; WestEberhard 1987), birds (e.g., responses of parents to each other; Wright and Cuthill 1989), primates (e.g., reciprocal altruism; Brosnan and de Waal 2002), and obviously, humans. Not surprisingly, a large theoretical literature focuses on the evolutionary consequences of particular kinds of flexible (or conditional) behavior (e.g., Axelrod and Hamilton 1981;McNamara et al 1999; Lehmann and Keller 2006;Akçay et al 2009;Boyd et al 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consider Smith's (1776Smith's ( /2007) famous invisible hand, in which every individual acting for his own good produces-as unintended by-products-benefits for others. Psychologists, as well, often regard helpful acts as being selfishly motivated, whether to consciously achieve material outcomes as in 'calculated reciprocity' (Brosnan & de Waal 2002), or unconsciously as a means to achieve psychological benefits such as the 'warm glow' that comes from helping (Andreoni 1990). Concern for the well-being of others is not necessary for prosocial behaviour (though Smith did pay special attention to these moral emotions ;Smith 1759Smith /2005.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, males may groom females to increase their chances of mating, or they may share meat according to the amount of help provided in hunting the prey (de Waal, 1989;Mitani & Watts, 2001), which would be in line with the reciprocal altruism hypothesis as originally formulated by Trivers (1971). Although some authors have maintained that chimpanzees keep a record of social credits and debits (Brosnan & de Waal, 2002), others argue that the correlations observed in the reciprocity of the positive behavior are not sufficient to draw conclusions about the calculation that would underlie this reciprocity (Hemelrijk, 1996).…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%