The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
2021
DOI: 10.1525/collabra.25293
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Practical Guide to Doing Behavioral Research on Fake News and Misinformation

Abstract: Coincident with the global rise in concern about the spread of misinformation on social media, there has been influx of behavioral research on so-called “fake news” (fabricated or false news headlines that are presented as if legitimate) and other forms of misinformation. These studies often present participants with news content that varies on relevant dimensions (e.g., true v. false, politically consistent v. inconsistent, etc.) and ask participants to make judgments (e.g., accuracy) or choices (e.g., whethe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
62
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 73 publications
(77 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
0
62
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Study 6 had two goals: (i) to replicate the findings from study 1 (the emotional language video) 1 year after it was originally conducted and (ii) to check whether manipulating the order in which participants respond to the outcome measures for each of the stimuli (technique recognition, trustworthiness, and sharing) influences the results. This is important because eliciting (for example) the manipulativeness and/or trustworthiness of a survey item before willingness to share might influence the responses participants give for the sharing measure, as participants may be primed to think about the item’s manipulativeness before providing their sharing intentions ( 29 , 30 ). Therefore, alongside the experimental condition, participants were randomly assigned to one of three response orders [manipulativeness – trustworthiness – sharing (MTS), n = 364; trustworthiness – sharing – manipulativeness (TSM), n = 361; or sharing – manipulativeness – trustworthiness (SMT); n = 343]; see Materials and Methods for more details.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Study 6 had two goals: (i) to replicate the findings from study 1 (the emotional language video) 1 year after it was originally conducted and (ii) to check whether manipulating the order in which participants respond to the outcome measures for each of the stimuli (technique recognition, trustworthiness, and sharing) influences the results. This is important because eliciting (for example) the manipulativeness and/or trustworthiness of a survey item before willingness to share might influence the responses participants give for the sharing measure, as participants may be primed to think about the item’s manipulativeness before providing their sharing intentions ( 29 , 30 ). Therefore, alongside the experimental condition, participants were randomly assigned to one of three response orders [manipulativeness – trustworthiness – sharing (MTS), n = 364; trustworthiness – sharing – manipulativeness (TSM), n = 361; or sharing – manipulativeness – trustworthiness (SMT); n = 343]; see Materials and Methods for more details.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For study 6, we varied the outcome measure response order across participants, as one of the study’s goals was to check whether the order in which participants respond to the measures beneath each of the stimuli has any bearing on the results ( 29 , 30 ). Participants were randomly assigned to one of three response orders (after being randomly assigned to an experimental condition): MTS ( n = 364), TSM ( n = 361), or SMT ( n = 343).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Researchers could also modify presentations of the vignette content to offer them in different modalities or information environments (Corneille et al, 2020 ; Fazio, Dolan, & Marsh, 2015b ). For example, with growing interest in people’s uptake of information online, the vignettes could be adapted as short-form videos (e.g., Butler et al, 2009 ), as posts on social media (e.g., Pennycook et al, 2021a ), or stripped of a narrative component completely (e.g., Fazio, Dolan, & Marsh, 2015b ; Salovich et al, 2022 ). These kinds of manipulations are possible with the current materials as they are intentionally brief, simple, flexible, allow for participant accountability, and are realistic.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, the stimuli used in survey experiments are typically collected, or even created, by the researchers. Thus, these posts may differ in important ways from the content that users would actually experience on their own social-media newsfeeds (Pennycook, Binnendyk, et al, 2021). For example, posts in survey experiments are typically presented without the social context that is such a core feature of social media (e.g., information about which user shared the content and which other users liked or commented on the content).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%