2007
DOI: 10.1093/brain/awm234
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A non-spatial bias favouring fixated stimuli revealed in patients with spatial neglect

Abstract: The cardinal feature of spatial neglect is severely impaired exploration of the contralesional space, a failure resulting in unawareness of many contralesional stimuli. This deficit is exacerbated by a reflexive attentional bias toward ipsilesional items. Here we show that, in addition to these spatially lateralized failures, neglect patients also exhibit a severe bias favouring stimuli presented at fixation. We tested neglect patients and matched healthy and right-hemisphere damaged patients without neglect i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

2
31
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 43 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
2
31
0
Order By: Relevance
“…similar durations were observed in a previous study when a simultanagnosic patient was required to make saccades to peripheral targets while a central fixation stimulus was present (a so-called overlap task; Nyffeler et al 2005). however, the sudden appearance of a stimulus at fixation also strongly affects fixation durations in patients with unilateral posterior brain damage and spatial neglect (Ptak et al 2007;Walker and Findlay 1996), while the prolonged durations during visual scanning appear to be specific to Bálint syndrome. how can 'sticky' fixation during ocular exploration be explained?…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…similar durations were observed in a previous study when a simultanagnosic patient was required to make saccades to peripheral targets while a central fixation stimulus was present (a so-called overlap task; Nyffeler et al 2005). however, the sudden appearance of a stimulus at fixation also strongly affects fixation durations in patients with unilateral posterior brain damage and spatial neglect (Ptak et al 2007;Walker and Findlay 1996), while the prolonged durations during visual scanning appear to be specific to Bálint syndrome. how can 'sticky' fixation during ocular exploration be explained?…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…For the number of saccades made during free exploration, we found direction-speciWc eVects: the number of saccades was signiWcantly reduced for leftward saccades and signiWcantly increased for rightward saccades. Studies in neglect patients examining saccade planning towards unilaterally presented visual targets reported signiWcantly decreased amplitude of contralesional saccades, but unaVected ipsilesional saccades (Heide & Kömpf, 1998;Ptak, Schnider, Golay, & Müri, 2007;Walker & Findlay, 1996). Furthermore, neglect patients are often slower to initiate contralesional saccades or making small, multistep saccades (Walker & Findlay, 1996;Behrmann, Ghiselli-Crippa & Dimatteo, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…All patients had preserved visual fields for the central ϳ20°as assessed with computerized perimetry testing (white dot presented on black background at positions ranging between Ϫ25 and ϩ25 degrees) and/or clinical confrontation. All neglect patients manifested behavioral signs of left unawareness (e.g., failure to notice objects or persons placed on their left, difficulties with dressing or grooming) and lateralized failures in the following neglect tests: Bells cancellation (Gauthier et al, 1989), cancellation of inverted Ts (Ptak et al, 2007), line bisection (Schenkenberg et al, 1980), sentence copying (Wilson et al, 1987), and copying a landscape. Participants had similar age (ANOVA across all four groups, F (3,36) ϭ 0.44), and the three patient groups had comparable time since injury (F (2,27) ϭ 0.45).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%