2004
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000904006336
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A multiple process solution to the logical problem of language acquisition

Abstract: Many researchers believe that there is a logical problem at the center of language acquisition theory. According to this analysis, the input to the learner is too inconsistent and incomplete to determine the acquisition of grammar. Moreover, when corrective feedback is provided, children tend to ignore it. As a result, language learning must rely on additional constraints from universal grammar. To solve this logical problem, theorists have proposed a series of constraints and parameterizations on the form of … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
118
0
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 222 publications
(120 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
(37 reference statements)
1
118
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Nonetheless, the above findings provide substantial motivation for further developing usage-based accounts of language acquisition. This optimism is supported by demonstrations of the functional underpinnings of anaphora constraints (van Hoek, 1997;Harris and Bates 2002) and proposals that these need not be taken as a priori unlearnable (Akhtar et al 2004;Lidz et al 2003;;MacWhinney, 2004). The proposal put forward here is that children's knowledge of anaphora constraints depends on understanding i) the relative accessibility of di¤erent referring expressions ii) the contexts in which referring expressions occur, particularly in within-sentence grammatical hierarchies and iii) the contrastive values of pronouns and reflexives.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…Nonetheless, the above findings provide substantial motivation for further developing usage-based accounts of language acquisition. This optimism is supported by demonstrations of the functional underpinnings of anaphora constraints (van Hoek, 1997;Harris and Bates 2002) and proposals that these need not be taken as a priori unlearnable (Akhtar et al 2004;Lidz et al 2003;;MacWhinney, 2004). The proposal put forward here is that children's knowledge of anaphora constraints depends on understanding i) the relative accessibility of di¤erent referring expressions ii) the contexts in which referring expressions occur, particularly in within-sentence grammatical hierarchies and iii) the contrastive values of pronouns and reflexives.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…MOSAIC is therefore often regarded as a usage-based model of language learning (cf. Goldberg, 1995;Lieven, Pine & Baldwin, 1997;Pine, Lieven & Rowland, 1998;Tomasello, 2000;, MacWhinney, 2004. However, although the mechanisms implemented in MOSAIC are certainly consistent with a usage-based analysis, it is important to recognise that MOSAIC is a relatively simple distributional analyser, with no access to semantic information, which is not sufficiently powerful to acquire many aspects of adult syntax.…”
Section: An Alternative Account Of Optional Infinitive Errorsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such overgeneralizations are problematic, because there is no direct competitor in the input which could block the incorrect use. Many authors (e.g., Clark, 1987;Goldberg, 1995;Macwhinney, 2004) have argued that somewhat indirect competitors (e.g., in this case, a periphrastic causative such as make disappear) are sufficient to block the overgeneralized form. If this is the case, however, it is unclear how the child could learn that certain periphrastic causatives [e.g., John made the baby stand up (e.g., through giving an order)] do not block the corresponding transitive causative sentence [e.g., John stood the baby up (e.g., by propping it up against a wall); examples from Bowerman, 1988].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%