2018
DOI: 10.1101/341206
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Model Quantitative Assessment Tool for Nonhuman Primate Environmental Enrichment Plans

Abstract: The housing and care of captive nonhuman primates (NHP) typically meets federal regulations and standards as well as guidelines by private accreditation organizations. There is, however, a gap between such policy, common practices, and the findings of a large empirical research literature on the effects of environmental enrichment (EE), particularly with respect to the degree to which different enrichment strategies lead to a demonstrable improvement of the animal's psychological wellbeing. Assessment tools to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

1
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 95 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In our study, however, participation did not decrease over time but actually increased across sessions. Even though animal task engagement does not necessarily provide evidence of a positive effect of the task on their welfare, it surely constitutes a basic indicator of enrichment success [25]. Therefore, the fact that participation increased across sessions indicates that chimpanzees found the termite mounds rewarding, but also that they likely required time to habituate to the task and become efficient at extracting the food.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In our study, however, participation did not decrease over time but actually increased across sessions. Even though animal task engagement does not necessarily provide evidence of a positive effect of the task on their welfare, it surely constitutes a basic indicator of enrichment success [25]. Therefore, the fact that participation increased across sessions indicates that chimpanzees found the termite mounds rewarding, but also that they likely required time to habituate to the task and become efficient at extracting the food.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In order to assess the impact of a particular enrichment strategy, researchers usually monitor their subjects' behavior by comparing the frequency of species-typical behaviors and abnormal/stress-related behaviors in the presence (versus in the absence) of the enrichment [16,23]. To date, a wide range of environmental enrichment strategies has been implemented and evaluated in non-human primates, including enclosure design and size, food novelty, foraging devices, computer-based devices, sensory stimulation or exposure to conspecifics or humans (i.e., social enrichment) [24,25]. Because wild non-human primates usually spend more time foraging or feeding than captive primates, it is not surprising that enrichment activities for captive groups have mainly focused on increasing opportunities for foraging or feeding [26][27][28].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Such an approach may allow for the identification of relevant resources or combinations of resources necessary for improving the welfare of laboratory housed rodents. It may also lend insight into relevant mental or cognitive capacities underlying species-typical behaviors (e.g., learning, memory, spatial navigation)—permitting for the development of innovative housing solutions that recapitulate the use of these capacities in the laboratory (for review, see Bennett et al, 2018 ; and for examples of evaluation and application, Dutton et al, 2018 ). In the 70s and 80s, such an approach was pursued in a range of farm animals (e.g., laying hens: Fölsch et al, 1983 ; rabbits: Stauffacher, 1992 ; pigs: Stolba and Wood-Gush, 1984 ), resulting in welfare-friendly prototypes of housing conditions, from which practicable solutions were further developed for specific contexts of animal use (e.g., laboratory rabbits: Stauffacher, 1994 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For this reason, environmental enrichment has become a key component of the management of captive animals (Maple and Perdue 2013 ), as it is considered an important means of improving animal welfare by providing opportunities for physical, affective and cognitive stimulation (Fernández and Martin 2021 ; Hoy et al 2010 ; Mellor 2015 ). The extensive variety of enrichment strategies used in non-human primates includes sensory stimulation (Carter et al 2021 ; Vaglio et al 2021 ), social housing (Chipangura et al 2020 ), motor or manipulative engagement (Costa et al 2018 ), and more recently, cognitive stimulation (Coleman and Novak 2017 ; Dutton et al 2018 ; Lutz and Novak 2005 ), which also includes the use of digital electronic devices (Clark 2017 ; Clark et al 2019 ; Gray et al 2018 ; Grunauer and Walguarnery 2018 ; Kim-McCormack et al 2016 ). In fact, cognitive enrichment has become increasingly popular in zoos, sanctuaries and even farms, where problem-solving opportunities can enhance animal welfare (Clark 2017 ; Meehan and Mench 2007 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%