2005
DOI: 10.1525/mp.2005.22.4.663
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Model of Melodic Expectation

Abstract: JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
93
0
2

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
3
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 129 publications
(102 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
(53 reference statements)
1
93
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Because musical tension is strongly linked to processes such as expectancy build-up, violation or fulfillment of expectancies, to the anticipation of resolution after a breach of expectancy, and to the eventual resolution of such a breach, musical tension plays an important role in the emotional aspects of music listening (see also Huron, 2008;Koelsch, 2012;Margulis, 2005;Meyer, 1956;Narmour, 1992;Rohrmeier & Koelsch, 2012). This is corroborated by empirical research showing that subjective ratings of musical tension correlate with ratings of discrete emotions (sadness, fear, and happiness) and physiological responses during music listening (Krumhansl, 1997).…”
Section: A Striking Feature Of Music Is Its Abilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because musical tension is strongly linked to processes such as expectancy build-up, violation or fulfillment of expectancies, to the anticipation of resolution after a breach of expectancy, and to the eventual resolution of such a breach, musical tension plays an important role in the emotional aspects of music listening (see also Huron, 2008;Koelsch, 2012;Margulis, 2005;Meyer, 1956;Narmour, 1992;Rohrmeier & Koelsch, 2012). This is corroborated by empirical research showing that subjective ratings of musical tension correlate with ratings of discrete emotions (sadness, fear, and happiness) and physiological responses during music listening (Krumhansl, 1997).…”
Section: A Striking Feature Of Music Is Its Abilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Active music listening involves building complex cognitive representations of musical structure (e.g., Koelsch, Rohrmeier, Torrecuso, & Jentschke, 2013;Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983;Patel, 2008), generating musical expectancies (e.g., Huron, 2006;Margulis, 2005;Meyer, 1956;Rohrmeier & Koelsch, 2012), and detecting and resolving musical ambiguities (e.g., Jackendoff, 1991;Slevc & Okada, 2015); all processes that plausibly draw on EFs. Producing and learning music likely involves even greater EF demands.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…From this beginning, many experiments based on the I-R model ensued (e.g., Cuddy & Lunney, 1995;Krumhansl, 1991bKrumhansl, , 1995aKrumhansl, , 1995bKrumhansl, , 1997Krumhansl et al, 2000;Pearce & Wiggins, 2006;Schellenberg 1996Schellenberg , 1997Schellenberg et al, 2002;Schmuckler, 1989;Thompson, Balkwill, & Vernescu, 2000;Thompson, Cuddy, & Plaus, 1997;Thompson & Stanton, 1998). For other important studies on melodic implication, see Carlsen (1981), Larson (2004), Margulis (2005), and Unyk and Carlsen (1987). following way.…”
Section: An Analytical Examplementioning
confidence: 97%