1997
DOI: 10.3758/bf03209391
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A model for recognition memory: REM—retrieving effectively from memory

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

55
1,232
7
8

Year Published

2002
2002
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 859 publications
(1,302 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
(37 reference statements)
55
1,232
7
8
Order By: Relevance
“…The experiments in this paper have applied the MCMC method to categorization tasks, but it could also be used in a variety of other domains as well. Many formal models of human cognition (Oaksford & Chater, 1998;Chater, Tenenbaum, & Yuille, 2006;Anderson, 1990;Shiffrin & Steyvers, 1997) assume that there is a range of degrees of belief in different hypothetical outcomes. The MCMC method is designed to estimate these subjective functions.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The experiments in this paper have applied the MCMC method to categorization tasks, but it could also be used in a variety of other domains as well. Many formal models of human cognition (Oaksford & Chater, 1998;Chater, Tenenbaum, & Yuille, 2006;Anderson, 1990;Shiffrin & Steyvers, 1997) assume that there is a range of degrees of belief in different hypothetical outcomes. The MCMC method is designed to estimate these subjective functions.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At a behavioral level, establishing that one particular type of encoding task retards memory in a distinct manner is challenging. In our view, the best way to make progress on such issues is by leveraging multiple sources of data including insight from cognitive neuroscience about the role of semantic memory in episodic encoding (Tse et al, 2007;Gliga, Volein, & Csibra, 2010), and by building and testing detailed computational models that triangulate between the multiple factors that influence memory performance (Shiffrin & Steyvers, 1997).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This comparison can take many forms, including a dot product (Hintzman, 1988) or likelihood ratio (Shiffrin & Steyvers, 1997;McClelland & Chappell, 1998); in any case, the comparison results in a single value reflecting the degree to which each memory trace is similar to the memory probe. This comparison is often assumed to take place in parallel across all memory traces (Gillund & Shiffrin, 1984;Hintzman, 1988;Shiffrin & Steyvers, 1997;McClelland & Chappell, 1998), but may take place over the course of a serial search among the various memory traces (Sternberg, 1966) or via a combination of parallel matching and serial search (Atkinson & Juola, 1974). Finally, these various match values are treated as evidence for whether the test item was or was not experienced in the target study context:…”
Section: Separate and Composite Representationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, alternative feature representations may be used that allow for much richer information to be encoded without altering the general structure of the model, such as the assumption of a geometric rather than Bernoulli distribution of feature values in the original REM model (Shiffrin & Steyvers, 1997). The SLiM model (McClelland & Chappell, 1998), like ours, assumes that probe features are binary, but allows for memory traces to contain continuous feature values in the range (0, 1).…”
Section: Alternative Memory Representationsmentioning
confidence: 99%