2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2019.104032
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A matter of priorities: High working memory enables (slightly) superior value-directed remembering

Abstract: People with larger working memory capacity exhibit enhanced free recall but do not show any advantage on tests of recognition. This pair of results suggests that differences in the strategies that people bring to the task of learning and retrieving are superior in learners with high working memory. There is ample evidence that learners with high working memory do indeed bring better strategies to both encoding and retrieval, but as yet little evidence of whether higher working memory is related to greater effe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
7
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
(51 reference statements)
1
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Such selectivity is emphasized by participants' study decisions in Experiment 2, with higher magnitudes receiving more study visits (and thus more total study time) despite equivalent study time per visit with lower magnitudes. As such, these results add further evidence that participants are effective in prioritizing important information, consistent with a large body of work demonstrating preserved selectivity with various materials like unrelated word pairs (Ariel et al, 2015), name-face pairs (Hargis & Castel, 2017), medication side effects (Hargis & Castel, 2018), and item-location pairs (Siegel & Castel, 2018b), and under varying degrees of cognitive ability, like healthy aging (Castel et al, 2002;Hayes et al, 2013;Siegel & Castel, 2018a), Alzheimer's disease (Castel et al, 2009), attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Castel et al, 2011), and different working memory capacities (Griffin, Benjamin, Sahakyan, & Stanley, 2019;Middlebrooks et al, 2017;Miller, Gross, & Unsworth, 2019;Robison & Unsworth, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…Such selectivity is emphasized by participants' study decisions in Experiment 2, with higher magnitudes receiving more study visits (and thus more total study time) despite equivalent study time per visit with lower magnitudes. As such, these results add further evidence that participants are effective in prioritizing important information, consistent with a large body of work demonstrating preserved selectivity with various materials like unrelated word pairs (Ariel et al, 2015), name-face pairs (Hargis & Castel, 2017), medication side effects (Hargis & Castel, 2018), and item-location pairs (Siegel & Castel, 2018b), and under varying degrees of cognitive ability, like healthy aging (Castel et al, 2002;Hayes et al, 2013;Siegel & Castel, 2018a), Alzheimer's disease (Castel et al, 2009), attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Castel et al, 2011), and different working memory capacities (Griffin, Benjamin, Sahakyan, & Stanley, 2019;Middlebrooks et al, 2017;Miller, Gross, & Unsworth, 2019;Robison & Unsworth, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…However, selectivity was better in older adults than in younger adults and preserved in younger adults when their attention was divided. While these findings, in addition to previous work using value-directed remembering tasks, reveal much about learning processes, most work has limited their analyses to recall as a function of value and emphasised strategic encoding processes (e.g., Ariel et al, 2015;Castel et al, 2002;Griffin et al, 2019;Nguyen et al, 2019;Robison & Unsworth, 2017;Wong et al, 2018), leaving much of the dynamic retrieval process unknown.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…Although evaluating the PFR and CRPs has revealed much about the dynamics of free recall, analyses of value-directed remembering tasks have been primarily limited to the probability of recall based on the point values assigned to each word (e.g., Ariel et al, 2015; Castel et al, 2002; Griffin et al, 2019; Nguyen et al, 2019; Robison & Unsworth, 2017; Wong et al, 2018). Some work using value-directed remembering procedures suggests that younger adults tend to initiate recall with high-value compared with low-value words and that CRPs are similar in participants completing a value-directed remembering task compared with controls where words are not paired with point values (see Stefanidi et al, 2018).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead, it is a composite of cognitive processes such as shifting, monitoring, and inhibition that are driven by the ability to strategically direct attention and to efficiently manage attentional resources (Conway et al, 2001; Engle, 2002; Oberauer & Hein, 2012). In particular, working memory is crucial for executive attention by prioritizing information and remembering only the most relevant information (Griffin et al, 2019; Myers et al, 2017; Robison & Unsworth, 2017; Unsworth et al, 2019). Therefore, this research focuses on the trait of efficient management of attentional resources of working memory capacity.…”
Section: Theoretical Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%