2016
DOI: 10.1177/2331216516682698
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Loudness Model for Time-Varying Sounds Incorporating Binaural Inhibition

Abstract: This article describes a model of loudness for time-varying sounds that incorporates the concept of binaural inhibition, namely, that the signal applied to one ear can reduce the internal response to a signal at the other ear. For each ear, the model includes the following: a filter to allow for the effects of transfer of sound through the outer and middle ear; a short-term spectral analysis with greater frequency resolution at low than at high frequencies; calculation of an excitation pattern, representing th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
58
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(64 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
3
58
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The weak significant entrainment to channel-specific short-term loudness may reflect a genuine effect or it may reflect false positives, since the channel-specific short-term loudness is correlated with the channel-specific instantaneous loudness for the same channel ( ρ  = 0.9, 0.8, 0.8, 0.8, 0.8, 0.8, 0.7, 0.8 and 0.7 for channels 1–9 respectively), and, for the middle channels, the channel-specific short-term loudness is correlated with the overall short-term loudness ( ρ  = 0.9, 0.8 and 0.8 for channels 3–5 respectively). Hence the data do not allow a clear conclusion about whether channel-specific instantaneous loudness is summed across channels to give the overall instantaneous loudness and the overall short-term loudness is determined from the overall instantaneous loudness, as assumed in the loudness model of Glasberg and Moore (2002), or whether short-term loudness is determined separately for each channel from the instantaneous loudness for that channel, and then short-term loudness values are summed across channels to give the overall short-term loudness, as assumed in the models of Chalupper and Fastl (2002) and Moore et al. (2016).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The weak significant entrainment to channel-specific short-term loudness may reflect a genuine effect or it may reflect false positives, since the channel-specific short-term loudness is correlated with the channel-specific instantaneous loudness for the same channel ( ρ  = 0.9, 0.8, 0.8, 0.8, 0.8, 0.8, 0.7, 0.8 and 0.7 for channels 1–9 respectively), and, for the middle channels, the channel-specific short-term loudness is correlated with the overall short-term loudness ( ρ  = 0.9, 0.8 and 0.8 for channels 3–5 respectively). Hence the data do not allow a clear conclusion about whether channel-specific instantaneous loudness is summed across channels to give the overall instantaneous loudness and the overall short-term loudness is determined from the overall instantaneous loudness, as assumed in the loudness model of Glasberg and Moore (2002), or whether short-term loudness is determined separately for each channel from the instantaneous loudness for that channel, and then short-term loudness values are summed across channels to give the overall short-term loudness, as assumed in the models of Chalupper and Fastl (2002) and Moore et al. (2016).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…1) is not the only plausible sequence leading to the neural computation of overall short-term loudness. Short-term loudness may instead be derived separately for each channel, and these short-term loudness estimates may then be combined across channels to give the overall short-term loudness, as assumed in the models of Chalupper and Fastl (2002) and Moore et al. (2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…1a. For example, binaural inhibition is a common feature 55 , often occurring across multiple timescales 53 . However these models are typically designed with a focus on explaining perception across a range of frequencies (and for inputs of arbitrary frequency content), rather than attempting to understand performance on specific tasks (i.e.…”
Section: The Model Shares Features With Previous Binaural Models Prementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The same noise consisted in a Gaussian white noise lasting two seconds for the calibration task and five seconds in the tone detection task (the white noise has been generated only once and loaded during each procedure to ensure that all the participants were tested with the same noise; sampling rate: 44100 Hz). Both the tone and the white noise signals were normalized between -1 to 1 (arbitrary units) and the loudness of the resultant stimuli (white noise with embedded tone) was estimated at ~ 91 dB SPL sound pressure level (SPL) using inhouse Matlab codes (Moore et al, 2016). During the entire procedure, the auditory stimuli were displayed at constant ~72 dB SPL and across participants.…”
Section: Audio Tones and White Noisementioning
confidence: 99%