2012
DOI: 10.1242/jeb.068106
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A long-latency aversive learning mechanism enables locusts to avoid odours associated with the consequences of ingesting toxic food

Abstract: SUMMARYAvoiding food that contains toxins is crucial for the survival of many animals, particularly herbivores, because many plants defend themselves with toxins. Some animals can learn to avoid food containing toxins not through its taste but by the toxinsʼ effects following ingestion, though how they do so remains unclear. We studied how desert locusts (Schistocerca gregaria), which are generalist herbivores, form post-ingestive aversive memories and use them to make appropriate olfactory-based decisions in … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
38
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
1
38
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, the two phases differ in when they first express this aversion; solitarious locusts show a conditioned aversion 10 min after training, whereas their gregarious counterparts do not. Such a difference is probably related to a phase-specific difference in the acquisition mechanism [34]. …”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, the two phases differ in when they first express this aversion; solitarious locusts show a conditioned aversion 10 min after training, whereas their gregarious counterparts do not. Such a difference is probably related to a phase-specific difference in the acquisition mechanism [34]. …”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Being able to rapidly form aversive associations, rather than waiting to determine the consequences of ingestion, should help solitarious locusts maintain their narrow dietary preferences and avoid ingesting toxins [29, 30]. This rapid acquisition of aversive associations in solitarious locusts is probably taste-mediated, because memories acquired by postingestive feedback take longer to manifest [34]. Conversely, the absence of this rapid, taste-mediated aversive learning mechanism in gregarious locusts matches their broader diet and their active ingestion of toxic plants to acquire and maintain unpalatability [28–30].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This idea is supported by the fact that two studies of associative olfactory conditioning in honeybees have identified that bees previously fed solutions containing quinine or amygdalin are less likely to extend their proboscis towards odours predicting reward and less likely to feed (Ayestaran et al 2010; Wright et al 2010). Likewise, locusts that have been injected with the toxin, nicotine hydrogen tartrate, also learn to avoid odours associated with the consequences of toxin injection (Simoes et al 2012). Identification of the extent to which these toxins directly act on the nervous system, and whether there are specific mechanisms for directly detecting toxins in the brain or in other ganglia or organs will be the subject of future investigations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Like mammals, invertebrates can learn to avoid cues associated with the consumption of toxins (Dethier 1980; Lee and Bernays 1990; Raffa 1987; Simoes et al 2012). Furthermore, injection with lithium chloride (LiCl) (the canonical toxin used to produce malaise in vertebrates) causes crayfish to experience limb trembling, uncontrolled movements and periods of immobility (Arzuffi et al 2000), and locusts injected with nicotine hydrogen tartrate are more likely to vomit (Simoes et al 2012). Establishing whether there are behavioural reactions from intoxication that could be termed ‘malaise’ in invertebrates would pave the way for understanding the physiological mechanisms that produce these behaviours and for establishing whether they have an adaptive basis.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Octopamine, tyramine and expression levels of their receptors during crowding or isolation were analyzed by Student's t -test. The statistical analysis of olfactory preference was referred to the work about locust aversive learning48. In olfactory behavioral assay, G-tests for goodness-of-fit were used to determine the significance of the divergence from an expected 50% decision for the volatile arm or air control arm, and the counts of locusts preferring the volatile or air control were used for this analysis.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%