2007
DOI: 10.3758/bf03193599
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A little elaboration goes a long way: The role of generation in eyewitness suggestibility

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
17
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
1
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, such manipulation is very common when testing the suggestibility effect in a lab setting (e.g. Gordon & Shapiro, 2012;Lane & Zaragoza, 2007;McCloskey & Zaragoza, 1985). Using slides allows better control for variables such as information load, exposure time and presentation sequence, compared with a movie, which may have more ecological validation but may also add possible confounds.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, such manipulation is very common when testing the suggestibility effect in a lab setting (e.g. Gordon & Shapiro, 2012;Lane & Zaragoza, 2007;McCloskey & Zaragoza, 1985). Using slides allows better control for variables such as information load, exposure time and presentation sequence, compared with a movie, which may have more ecological validation but may also add possible confounds.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They first answered yes/no questions about the slide show, which introduced MPI for half of the target details. Finally, all of the participants were tested for their verbatim memory of the target details at the subordinate level, as in many misinformation studies (e.g., Eakin et al, 2003;Lane & Zaragoza, 2007;Lindsay & Johnson, 1989;Loftus et al, 1978;McCloskey & Zaragoza, 1985). The participants were also asked to indicate, for each of their responses, whether they remembered it from the slide show or were merely guessing.…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This was coined the “misinformation effect,” and since its observation, researchers have continued to expand the scope of this work. In all of these studies, the misinformation effect has proven to be a robust phenomenon; it occurs in participants of all ages (from preschoolers to older adults), when presented in a variety of different ways (e.g., narratives, post-event questions, imagination, or even self-generation), for both simulated and real-world events and across several different types of memory tests (e.g., recall, recognition, and source-monitoring) (Ackil and Zaragoza, 1995, 1998; Ceci et al, 1987; Drivdahl et al, 2009; Lane and Zaragoza, 2007; Lindsay, 1990; Nourkova et al, 2004; Zaragoza et al, 2011). Additionally, it has been reported that not only can memories for individual items be altered through misinformation, but memories for entire events can be fabricated by participants, either in conjunction with a previously shown witnessed event (Chrobak and Zaragoza, 2008) or within their own life history (Lindsay et al, 2004; Wade et al, 2002).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%