1994
DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.101.2.266
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A law of comparative judgment.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

4
814
0
20

Year Published

1998
1998
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 706 publications
(847 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
4
814
0
20
Order By: Relevance
“…These submodels result from specifying whether variance estimates differ within a dimension (i.e., Case V vs. Case III models) and whether variance estimates differ across dimensions (i.e., isotropic vs. anisotropic models). Following Thurstone's (1927) nomenclature, Case V models estimate the same variance for all stimuli within a dimension (e.g., variability in perception of body size is constant across stimuli), whereas the more general Case III models estimate a variance for each stimulus within a dimension (e.g., variability in perception of body size is stimulus specific). The second distinction specifies whether variances differ across dimensions.…”
Section: Probabilistic Mds Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These submodels result from specifying whether variance estimates differ within a dimension (i.e., Case V vs. Case III models) and whether variance estimates differ across dimensions (i.e., isotropic vs. anisotropic models). Following Thurstone's (1927) nomenclature, Case V models estimate the same variance for all stimuli within a dimension (e.g., variability in perception of body size is constant across stimuli), whereas the more general Case III models estimate a variance for each stimulus within a dimension (e.g., variability in perception of body size is stimulus specific). The second distinction specifies whether variances differ across dimensions.…”
Section: Probabilistic Mds Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is based on the premise that medical interventions are composed of a set of attributes or outcomes and that the attractiveness of a particular intervention to an individual is a function of these attributes. [13,14] Conjoint techniques are grounded in both psychology and economic theory[15,16] and offer several advantages over simple rating or ranking exercises, including the ability to examine the strength of individual preferences across treatment attributes and observe the willingness to trade between different attributes. [14,17] While the treatment combinations presented in our survey are hypothetical in nature, the specific features of the treatments reflect the characteristics of new DAAs that are of most importance to patients.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…L. Thurstone's (1927) model provides a powerful framework for modeling individual differences in choice behavior. An overview of Thurstonian models for comparative data is provided, including the classical Case V and Case III models as well as more general choice models with unrestricted and factor-analytic covariance structures.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent applications include visual paired-comparison studies involving young children (Pascalis, de Haan, & Nelson, 2002;Turati & Simion, 2002;Younger & Furrer, 2003), rankings of risk perceptions , food characteristics (Oakes & Slotterback, 2002), and clinical services (Hazell, Tarren-Sweeney, Vimpani, Keatinge, & Callan, 2002). Most statistical models for the analysis of paired-comparison and ranking data are based on Thurstone's (1927) work, which emphasized that decisions should be viewed as probabilistic to account for apparent inconsistencies in choice outcomes. For example, when confronted several times with the same choice alternative pair, respondents may not consistently choose the same choice alternative each time.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation