1998
DOI: 10.1515/9783110805536
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Grammar of Kambera

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
25
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 156 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The Kambera data are based on a corpus analysis by Onvlee (1984), a comprehensive Kambera-Dutch dictionary, while the Dutch data are based on a corpus analysis of the standard dictionary of Dutch (Van Dale Groot Woordenboek der Nederlandse Taal), and on an experiment with Dutch native speakers. The formal complexity of the items discussed here has been motivated independently in the literature on Kambera and Dutch (for Kambera, see van der Hulst & Klamer 1997a, 1997b, Klamer 1998for Dutch, see Booij 1995:35-43, and the references cited there). The semantic classifications of the items came from independent sources as well.…”
Section: Structural Complexity Correlates With Semantic Complexitymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The Kambera data are based on a corpus analysis by Onvlee (1984), a comprehensive Kambera-Dutch dictionary, while the Dutch data are based on a corpus analysis of the standard dictionary of Dutch (Van Dale Groot Woordenboek der Nederlandse Taal), and on an experiment with Dutch native speakers. The formal complexity of the items discussed here has been motivated independently in the literature on Kambera and Dutch (for Kambera, see van der Hulst & Klamer 1997a, 1997b, Klamer 1998for Dutch, see Booij 1995:35-43, and the references cited there). The semantic classifications of the items came from independent sources as well.…”
Section: Structural Complexity Correlates With Semantic Complexitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…), data from prosody, reduplication, and language games indicate that the final consonant is not fully integrated into the prosodic template that is generally applicable for Kambera root forms-the trochaic foot. (Evidence and independent motivation of the prosodic complexity of (C)VCVC roots is given in van der Hulst & Klamer 1997a, 1997band Klamer 1998. In other words, though Kambera consonant-final roots are morphologically simple (they are morphological roots, not derived words), they are prosodically complex, because they consist of a foot combined with an additional consonant.…”
Section: Structural Complexity Correlates With Semantic Complexitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Beneficiaries/Recipients are regularly marked with a dative clitic, as in (4) (cf. Klamer, 1998, for details):3 (3) Da-ngarrdi -ya na uhu 3pN-take -3sA ART rice 'They take the rice' (4) Da-ngarrdi -nya na uhu i Ama 3pN-take -3sD ART rice ART father 'They bring father the rice'…”
Section: Kambera Report Constructionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kambera and Buru are both classified as Central MalayoPolynesian languages. Kambera has 150,000 speakers, is spoken on the island of Sumba in Eastern Indonesia, and described in Klamer (1998). Buru has 43,000 speakers, is spoken in Central Maluku, Indonesia, and described in Grimes (1991).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the literature it has been referred to as 'Sumbaneesch' (Wielenga 1909), 'Sumba(a)sch' (Onvlee 1925), 'Kamberaas' (Onvlee 1984), and 'Bahasa Sumba/Kambera' (Kapita 1982). Klamer (1998a) is a recent grammar of the language. 3 The information presented in this chapter is based on a corpus of 12-hours of spontaneous speech, plus additional elicitation, collected in Sumba during 12 months of fieldwork between 1991 and 1994 in one village, Katàka.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%