2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.jmp.2019.03.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A geometric framework for modeling dynamic decisions among arbitrarily many alternatives

Abstract: Multiple-choice and continuous-response tasks pose a number of challenges for models of the decision process, from empirical challenges like context effects to computational demands imposed by choice sets with a large number of outcomes. This paper develops a general framework for constructing models of the cognitive processes underlying both inferential and preferential choice among any arbitrarily large number of alternatives. This geometric approach represents the alternatives in a choice set along with a d… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
51
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

5
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(63 citation statements)
references
References 100 publications
0
51
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Such models, despite being much more complex than those presented here, offer many potential benefits. For example, they can be used to predict the effect of condition manipulations (e.g., different sets of colors in the Stroop task) on accuracy and response times in decision tasks (Bhatia, 2017;Kvam, 2019b), which makes them well suited to identify correspondence between different behavioral tasks (e.g., Stroop, Flanker). Indeed, many generative and cognitive models are developed to jointly capture phenomena across paradigmsa process that often produces mechanistic insights that are easily obscured when using summary statistics (e.g., Kellen et al, 2016;Luckman et al, 2018;Turner et al, 2018).…”
Section: Further Improvementsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such models, despite being much more complex than those presented here, offer many potential benefits. For example, they can be used to predict the effect of condition manipulations (e.g., different sets of colors in the Stroop task) on accuracy and response times in decision tasks (Bhatia, 2017;Kvam, 2019b), which makes them well suited to identify correspondence between different behavioral tasks (e.g., Stroop, Flanker). Indeed, many generative and cognitive models are developed to jointly capture phenomena across paradigmsa process that often produces mechanistic insights that are easily obscured when using summary statistics (e.g., Kellen et al, 2016;Luckman et al, 2018;Turner et al, 2018).…”
Section: Further Improvementsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In many ways, the GSR model's stopping rule is similar to the rule in the CDM and SCDM in that it will halt as soon as evidence for any of the alternatives in the choice set exceeds the threshold. In fact, the distribution of response times in the GSR model will follow exactly the first-passage distribution for hitting times on the hypersphere in d dimensions derived by Smith & Corbett (2018), and the raw hitting locations will follow a von Mises-Fisher distribution (Kvam, 2019a).The key extension provided by the GSR model is that not every hitting point on the hypersphere corresponds to a unique response option. Instead, the distribution of hitting points is re-mapped onto the set of response options by interpolating the location of a response from the locations of a more limited set of responses along the hypersphere.…”
Section: Formal Specification Of the Gsr Modelmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…There is a growing consensus that the evidence accumulates gradually and sequentially to make a decision ( 29,30 ). As a result, sequential sampling models (SSMs; Stone, 1960 ( 31 ), Ratcliff, Smith, Brown, and McKoon, 2016 ( 32 ) and Evans and Wagenmakers, 2020 ( 33 ) for the reviews), as the most well-known explanation of how the decision-making process works ( 26,30 ), have obtained very achievements in modeling the cognitive processes underlying decision making across a wide variety of paradigms, such as optimality polices ( [34][35][36][37] ), stop signal paradigms ( 38 ), go/no-go paradigms ( 39 ), multi-attribute & many alternatives choice ( [40][41][42] ), learning strategies ( [43][44][45] ), attentional choice ( [8][9][10]46 ), continuous responses ( 29,47 ), neural processes ( 1 ), and so on. In general, SSMs assume that decisions are made from a noisy process of accumulating evidence, that is to say, according to this theory, the evidence is gradually accumulated in favor of different choice alternatives over time with some rate until a sufficient amount of evidence for one of the options reaches a predetermined threshold to make a decision.…”
Section: Cognitive Modeling Of Perceptual Decisionsmentioning
confidence: 99%