2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.rasd.2015.04.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A further evaluation of the effects of listener training on derived categorization and speaker behavior in children with autism

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
26
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
2
26
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The fact that she vocalized in only 56 % of trials, and yet responded at a high accuracy in MTS suggests that either she did not use the verbal strategy in some trials, or did so covertly. P4's performance supports previous studies (e.g., Kobari-Wright and Miguel 2014;Miguel et al 2008;Lee et al 2015;Miguel and Kobari-Wright 2013) that found that when participants are required to tact the sample, their visual-visual MTS performance improves as tacting stimuli may serve to generate the discriminative stimulus necessary to evoke the selection response.…”
Section: [A'] Goes With [B'])supporting
confidence: 83%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The fact that she vocalized in only 56 % of trials, and yet responded at a high accuracy in MTS suggests that either she did not use the verbal strategy in some trials, or did so covertly. P4's performance supports previous studies (e.g., Kobari-Wright and Miguel 2014;Miguel et al 2008;Lee et al 2015;Miguel and Kobari-Wright 2013) that found that when participants are required to tact the sample, their visual-visual MTS performance improves as tacting stimuli may serve to generate the discriminative stimulus necessary to evoke the selection response.…”
Section: [A'] Goes With [B'])supporting
confidence: 83%
“…This is because the picture of the poodle already occasioned listener behavior. This interpretation is also corroborated by improvements in categorization performance when participants were required to tact the samples (e.g., Lee et al 2015;Lowe et al 2002;Miguel et al 2008;Miguel and Kobari-Wright 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 72%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Once interdependence between speaker and listener repertoires (i.e., naming) is established, either training condition will simultaneously establish both tact and listener behavior (Horne & Lowe, 1996). While preliminary findings suggest this interdependence begins to occur around 30 months in typical development (Lee et al, 2015), several studies involving children with disabilities have suggested that this interdependence can be established through specific training procedures, such as multiple exemplar instruction (Greer, Stolfi, Chavez-Brown, & Rivera-Valdes, 2005;Fiorile & Greer, 2007;Greer, Stolfi, & Pistoljevic, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…Subsequent reinforcement will simultaneously strengthen the tact as well as the listener behavior of orienting to the ball upon hearing the word Bball.^Horne and Lowe (1996) proposed that this interdependence allows children to acquire both tact and listener responses through instruction in either form. This suggestion has been supported in a variety of experimental studies with typically developing children (e.g., Lowe, Horne, Harris, & Randle, 2002;Horne, Lowe, & Randle, 2004;Lowe, Horne, & Hughes, 2005;Miguel, Petursdottir, Carr, & Michael, 2008) and children with ASD (e.g., Miguel & Kobari-Wright, 2013;Kobari-Wright & Miguel, 2014;Lee, Miguel, Darcey, & Jennings, 2015). Naming also allows children to acquire tact and listener skills through language pairings (e.g., the instructor labels an object in the presence of the learner), as demonstrated in experimental studies with typically developing children (e.g., Gilic & Greer, 2011;Longano & Greer, 2014) and children with ASD (e.g., Fiorile & Greer, 2007;Longano & Greer, 2014).…”
Section: Implications For Practicementioning
confidence: 73%