1983
DOI: 10.3758/bf03213471
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A direct comparison of recognition failure rates for recallable names in episodic and semanticmemory tests

Abstract: In an extension of Muter's (l978l research, subjects studied pairs of lowercase cues and uppercase targets consisting of famous names (e.g., betsy ROSS), nonfamous names (e.g., edwin CONWAY), weakly related words (e.g., grasp BABY), and unrelated words (e.g., art GO). Following recognition tests in which surname and word targets were tested in the absence of their cues, cued recall tests for the surname and word targets were given. In semantic recognition and recall tests, the response to a surname was to be m… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
58
0

Year Published

1989
1989
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 85 publications
(62 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
4
58
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The effects of reading a name on a later judgment of its fame largely replicate results reported by Neely and Payne (1983). Those effects remain unchanged when attention is divided while one reads names, even though divided attention, in comparison with full attention, has a large effect on list-recognition performance.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 75%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The effects of reading a name on a later judgment of its fame largely replicate results reported by Neely and Payne (1983). Those effects remain unchanged when attention is divided while one reads names, even though divided attention, in comparison with full attention, has a large effect on list-recognition performance.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 75%
“…Then those old names were mixed with new famous and new nonfamous names in a test of fame judgments. On the basis of earlier research (Jacoby, Kelley, Brown, & Jasechko, 1989;Neely & Payne, 1983), we expected that the familiarity of a name produced by its being read in the first phase would be mistaken for the familiarity that characterizes a famous name; that is, reading either a famous name or a nonfamous name in the first part of the experiment would produce a higher probability of calling the name "famous" on the later test. We expected that divided attention would reduce people's ability to recognize a name as previously read but would have no effect on gains in the familiarity of the name produced by its prior reading.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The major difference in the current experiments had to do with the instructions given to subjects at the time of the second test--cued recall versus word completion--while holding the physical form of the retrieval cues constant (cf. Graf, Squire, & Mandler, 1984;Neely & Payne, 1983). The retrieval cues were graphemic word fragments, that is, words with a number of letters deleted from various positions in a word (e.g., Tulving et al, 1982).…”
Section: Contingent Dissociation and The Methods Of Triangulationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The scoring of the subjects' performance in the two retrieval tasks was also identical: Target words for which subjects were given credit were the words that had appeared on the study list. Our general plan for experiments conforms to the methodological suggestions made by Neely and Payne (1983) and follows the example set by and .…”
Section: Overview Of Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Priming paradigms also have been used to look for dissociative effects, whereby a variable produces different effects in episodic and semantic tasks when all other factors are held constant. Evidence of dissociative effects can be used to argue in favor of the distinction (Neely & Payne, 1983), while failures of dissociation constitute evidence against the distinction (Yantis & Meyer, 1988). Although the present study is not intended as a test of the episodic-semantic distinction per se, it can be construed as looking for evidence of episodic priming in a semantic task.…”
Section: Studies Of the Episodic-semantic Distinctionmentioning
confidence: 99%