Conversation Analysis 2009
DOI: 10.1017/cbo9780511635670.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A cross-linguistic investigation of the site of initiation in same-turn self-repair

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
15
0
3

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
15
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Nooteboom (2005aNooteboom ( , 2010 takes evidence of a bimodal distribution of reparandum lengths, which has also been observed in conversation-analytic studies of repair (Schegloff 1979, Fox et al 2009), as support for a qualitative difference between early and late repairs. It is certainly difficult to see why, for example, 10% or 90% complete target word attempts should be attested more frequently than 50% complete target word attempts if repair is initiated immediately upon error detection by a single monitoring process.…”
Section: The Influence Of Repair Timingmentioning
confidence: 55%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Nooteboom (2005aNooteboom ( , 2010 takes evidence of a bimodal distribution of reparandum lengths, which has also been observed in conversation-analytic studies of repair (Schegloff 1979, Fox et al 2009), as support for a qualitative difference between early and late repairs. It is certainly difficult to see why, for example, 10% or 90% complete target word attempts should be attested more frequently than 50% complete target word attempts if repair is initiated immediately upon error detection by a single monitoring process.…”
Section: The Influence Of Repair Timingmentioning
confidence: 55%
“…On the other 6 Nooteboom's finding tallies well with Schegloff's (1979: 275) observation that in English spontaneous talk-ininteraction, repair is commonly initiated 'after the first sound of a word or just before its last sound'. Fox et al (2009) show that this tendency can be observed to some extent across a number of genetically unrelated languages. 7 Oomen & Postma (2001: 173) similarly operationalise what they call the 'error to cutoff time' as the duration from the start of the lexical item that is subject to repair to its end even though in their data, too, the point of observable error in phonological error repairs must vary.…”
Section: Quantifying Repair Timingmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…the repairable, self-initiation, and self-repair segment, or in different next turns, while the latter, other repair or other initiation could only happen in the next turn, since it is from the other speaker. Though other initiation could also project self-repair, this study particularly selects those self-initiated self-repair turns as the focus in which laughter is employed between the post-initiation and pre-completion of the same turn, according to Fox, et al (2009) or between the repairable and the repairing segment according to Rieger (2003). These turns mostly occur in the Candidates' response to interviewer's question.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The most common location of repair initiation, according to Schegloff (1979), is just after the start of a turnconstructional unit (post-initiation) or just before its completion (pre-completion), for example, in the case of a word after its first sound or just before its last sound (Schegloff, 1979: p. 275). The relevant domain for the post-initiation (or post-beginning, as Fox et al, 2009 term it) of a unit starts after the first sound is recognizable and continues until the first sound is complete; whereas the relevant domain for pre-completion begins just before the final sound is articulated, and continues until just before the final sound is complete (Fox et al, 2009: p. 65). The term recognizable completion has been introduced by Fox et al (2009).…”
Section: Repair Initiationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation