2018
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00416
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Computational Model of Working Memory Integrating Time-Based Decay and Interference

Abstract: There is still a strong debate in the working memory literature about the cause of forgetting, with many articles providing evidence for the existence of temporal decay and as many publications providing evidence compatible with interference being the only mechanism involved in forgetting. In order to reconcile the two views, this article describes TBRS∗-I (for Time-Based Resource-Sharing∗-Interference), a computational model of working memory which incorporates an interference-based mechanism to the decay-bas… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 63 publications
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This interaction was detected for the information processing speed and working memory manipulations, but only when the manipulated process was the outcome (i.e., no cross-domain interaction effects that would suggest that impairments in one process are attributable to impairments in the other). These findings collectively indicate that (a) working memory deficits in ADHD cannot be explained by more basic task demands associated with the rate at which information is encoded, (b) the ADHD group’s reduced information processing speed may be most detectable under conditions that allow relatively fast information processing speed and place relatively low demands on working memory (despite being relatively unaffected by changes in inhibition demands; Fosco, Kofler, Alderson, et al, 2019), whereas conditions that evoke slower information processing speed/higher working memory demands differentially affect non-ADHD children who then perform more similarly to children with ADHD, and (c) children with ADHD have disproportionate difficulties retaining information across a delay, which occurs even when not faced with external distractors that produce interference effects and/or reallocate attention to prevent rehearsal of to-be-recalled stimuli (Bolden, Rapport, Raiker, Sarver, & Kofler, 2012; Lemaire & Portrat, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This interaction was detected for the information processing speed and working memory manipulations, but only when the manipulated process was the outcome (i.e., no cross-domain interaction effects that would suggest that impairments in one process are attributable to impairments in the other). These findings collectively indicate that (a) working memory deficits in ADHD cannot be explained by more basic task demands associated with the rate at which information is encoded, (b) the ADHD group’s reduced information processing speed may be most detectable under conditions that allow relatively fast information processing speed and place relatively low demands on working memory (despite being relatively unaffected by changes in inhibition demands; Fosco, Kofler, Alderson, et al, 2019), whereas conditions that evoke slower information processing speed/higher working memory demands differentially affect non-ADHD children who then perform more similarly to children with ADHD, and (c) children with ADHD have disproportionate difficulties retaining information across a delay, which occurs even when not faced with external distractors that produce interference effects and/or reallocate attention to prevent rehearsal of to-be-recalled stimuli (Bolden, Rapport, Raiker, Sarver, & Kofler, 2012; Lemaire & Portrat, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, the cognitive load of the secondary task has an inverse relationship to the total number of items that can be maintained in working memory. There are several alternative models of cognitive load effects with somewhat different assumptions, but all of them predict a strong, inverse relationship between cognitive load and the total number of items remembered in dual-task situations (Lemaire & Portrat, 2018;Oberauer & Lewandowsky, 2011;Oberauer, Lewandowsky, Farrell, Jarrold, & Greaves, 2012;Portrat & Lemaire, 2015). Accordingly, the cognitive load effect has been proposed to be a Priority-A benchmark any model of working memory should be able to explain (Oberauer et al, 2018), in part because the effect appears to generalize across different experimental procedures and materials.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is easy to feel despondent about the state of short-term memory research: how can we have so much evidence yet so few firm conclusions? In light of this, there are grounds for thinking that a move away from a narrow systems-based approach to short-term memory may offer at the very least some new avenues of investigation, whether in the form of active inference frameworks [65], computational approaches [66], or simply better theory-neutral benchmarks for assessing competing models [67]. Likewise, while neuroscientific data has long played an important role in informing and constraining theoretical debates about short-term memory, one might hope that by identifying or modelling specific memory mechanisms -such as the oscillatory properties of neurons encoding working memory information [68] -new light may be shed on longstanding debates, especially in light of ongoing progress in neuroimaging techniques.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%