1990
DOI: 10.1016/0022-0965(90)90065-g
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A componential analysis of an early learning deficit in mathematics

Abstract: This study was designed to assess strategy choice and information-processing differences in normal and mathematically disabled first and second grade children. Twenty-three normal and 29 learning disabled (LD) children solved 40 computerpresented simple addition problems. Strategies, and their associated solution times, used in problem solving were recorded on a trial-by-trial basis and each was classified in accordance with the distributions of associations model of strategy choices. Based on performance in a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

17
281
4
25

Year Published

1991
1991
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 227 publications
(327 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
(65 reference statements)
17
281
4
25
Order By: Relevance
“…Recall that Cerella and Fozard found no age difference in the rate of retrieving the meaning of single words from long-term memory. In fact, data from converging areas, such as psychometrics, mental chronometry, learning disabilities, and neuropsychology, support the position that the long-term memory representation of arithmetic facts is a semantic language-like system (Ashcraft & Battaglia, 1978;Billet & Grafman, 1983;Geary, 1990;Geary & Brown, 1991;Geary et al, in press;Horn, 1968;Luria, 1980;Richman, 1983). Thus, the finding of no age difference in the rate of addition fact retrieval might be interpreted as a replication, albeit with a different content, of the Cerella and Fozard finding.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Recall that Cerella and Fozard found no age difference in the rate of retrieving the meaning of single words from long-term memory. In fact, data from converging areas, such as psychometrics, mental chronometry, learning disabilities, and neuropsychology, support the position that the long-term memory representation of arithmetic facts is a semantic language-like system (Ashcraft & Battaglia, 1978;Billet & Grafman, 1983;Geary, 1990;Geary & Brown, 1991;Geary et al, in press;Horn, 1968;Luria, 1980;Richman, 1983). Thus, the finding of no age difference in the rate of addition fact retrieval might be interpreted as a replication, albeit with a different content, of the Cerella and Fozard finding.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The final set of analyses in this section sought to determine if the use of the backup strategies was related to problem difficulty, as is the case for children (Geary, 1990;Geary & Brown, 1991;Siegler & Shrager, 1984). To achieve this end, the frequency (across problems) with which the backup strategies were used for problem solving was correlated with the value of the problem's correct sum.…”
Section: Strategy Choicesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…SpeciWcally, working memory is most important during the initial phase of arithmetic-skill acquisition and its role declines as procedures are used less frequently and facts become represented in long-term memory. Working-memory resources might thus be needed to achieve a complete representation of number facts in long-term memory (e.g., Geary, 1990;Geary & Brown, 1991;Hitch & McAuley, 1991;Siegler & Shrager, 1984), which explains the correlation between working-memory span and retrieval use in the younger children. However, once the number facts are completely represented in longterm memory, fact retrieval becomes more automatic and less eVortful, resulting in smaller arithmetic-performance diVerences between high-span children and low-span children.…”
Section: Diverences Between Addition and Multiplicationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Brown and Burton (1978) found in their study that the students forget the place they count after a while since they use immature strategies like finger counting, and therefore make this error. Geary (1990), Siegler and Robinson (1982) indicated that this error was particularly common in addition operations. Because students count as 4, 5, 6 instead of 5, 6, 7 in an addition operation of 4+3, they get the incorrect result.…”
Section: Discussion Conclusion and Suggestionsmentioning
confidence: 99%