1990
DOI: 10.1901/jaba.1990.23-197
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Collateral Effect of Reward Predicted by Matching Theory

Abstract: Matching theory describes a process by which organisms distribute their behavior between two or more concurrent schedules of reinforcement (Herrnstein, 1961). In an attempt to determine the generality of matching theory to applied settings, 2 students receiving special education were provided with academic response alternatives. Using a combined simultaneous treatments design and reversal design, unequal ratio schedules of reinforcement were varied across two academic responses. Findings indicated that both su… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
48
0

Year Published

1997
1997
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 71 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
(26 reference statements)
4
48
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These have a direct bearing on the validity of the relationship described in Equations I and 2 in human behavior. Three of these employed concurrent VI-VI schedules (Conger & Killeen, 1974;Mace, Neef, Shade, & Mauro, 1994;Neef, Mace, Shea, & Shade, 1992), while the others used Equation 2 to describe human behavior under VI schedules (Beardsley & McDowell, 1992;Mace, McCurdy, & Quigley, 1990;Martens, Halperin, Rummel, & Kilpatrick, 1990;Martens & Houk, 1989;Martens, Lochner, & Kelly, 1992;McDowell, 1982). The table shows that studies in naturalistic settings employed a wide range of response classes and consequences.…”
Section: Human Sensitivity To Relative Rate Of Reinforcement: Naturalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These have a direct bearing on the validity of the relationship described in Equations I and 2 in human behavior. Three of these employed concurrent VI-VI schedules (Conger & Killeen, 1974;Mace, Neef, Shade, & Mauro, 1994;Neef, Mace, Shea, & Shade, 1992), while the others used Equation 2 to describe human behavior under VI schedules (Beardsley & McDowell, 1992;Mace, McCurdy, & Quigley, 1990;Martens, Halperin, Rummel, & Kilpatrick, 1990;Martens & Houk, 1989;Martens, Lochner, & Kelly, 1992;McDowell, 1982). The table shows that studies in naturalistic settings employed a wide range of response classes and consequences.…”
Section: Human Sensitivity To Relative Rate Of Reinforcement: Naturalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Self-instruction 4. Self-reinforcement A behavioral view of self-regulation emphasizes the importance of having students learn to postpone an immediate reward in return for a more appropriate (and hopefully more satisfying) reward at a later time (Mace, Belfiore, & Shea, 1990). Self-regulation of behavior involves identifying behavioral alternatives, choosing reinforcers for the behavioral alternatives, and managing the delivery of delayed consequences.…”
Section: Student-directed Task Engagementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, alteration of response effort, reinforcement quality, and schedule values have been shown to similarly shift response allocation during concurrent-ratio schedules (DeLeon, Fisher, Herman, & Crosland, 2000;Perry & Fisher, 2001). Altering values of concurrent-ratio schedules has successfully decreased maladaptive behaviors while increasing functional behaviors, as well as showing control over the allocation of two functional behaviors (Horner & Day, 1991;Mace, McCurdy, & Quigley, 1990). Response allocation also shifts from a less to a more effortful response during a concurrent-ratio situation by increasing the rate of reinforcement for the more effortful response (Cuvo, Lerch, Leurquin, Gaffaney, & Poppen, 1998).…”
Section: Abstract: Concurrent Schedules; Ratio Schedules; Concurrent-mentioning
confidence: 99%