2018
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-018-0856-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A close call: Interference from semantic neighbourhood density and similarity in language production

Abstract: The present study investigated how lexical selection is influenced by the number of semantically related representations (semantic neighbourhood density) and their similarity (semantic distance) to the target in a speeded picture-naming task. Semantic neighbourhood density and similarity as continuous variables were used to assess lexical selection for which competitive and noncompetitive mechanisms have been proposed. Previous studies found mixed effects of semantic neighbourhood variables, leaving this issue… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
46
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(49 citation statements)
references
References 76 publications
2
46
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, as noted above Mirman (2011) andFieder, Wartenburger, andAbdel Rahman (2018) observed inhibitory influences of the number of closely related semantic neighbors (but see Bormann, 2011, for a null effect of overall semantic neighborhood size). Crucially, while this inhibitory influence is naturally predicted by language production theories assuming lexical competition, in the absence of external context stimuli semantic interference effects cannot be explained in terms of blocked articulators.…”
mentioning
confidence: 65%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Specifically, as noted above Mirman (2011) andFieder, Wartenburger, andAbdel Rahman (2018) observed inhibitory influences of the number of closely related semantic neighbors (but see Bormann, 2011, for a null effect of overall semantic neighborhood size). Crucially, while this inhibitory influence is naturally predicted by language production theories assuming lexical competition, in the absence of external context stimuli semantic interference effects cannot be explained in terms of blocked articulators.…”
mentioning
confidence: 65%
“…In conclusion, the present study focused on investigating the electrophysiological correlates and temporal dynamics of influences of message-inherent attributes of semantic richness and density. While item-inherent semantic influences on language production have recently attracted growing attention (Bormann, 2011;Fieder et al, 2018;Mirman, 2011;Rabovsky et al, 2016), their neural correlates and time courses have not yet been investigated.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…By contrast, Chen and Mirman (2012) suggested that strongly active neighbors exert a net interference effect, and weakly activated neighbors exert a net facilitative effect. Fieder et al (2018), however, found that the lexical cohort size (i.e., semantic neighborhood density) has a detrimental effect on picture naming for only the close semantic neighbors but not for the distant or the category-specific semantic neighbors. Different semantic contexts in different paradigms produce distinct results, and we thus suggest that the activation of the lexical cohort is flexible and dynamic to the message-inherent semantic attributes and the context in which the specific items are created during the experiment (see also Mirman, 2011).…”
Section: The Lexical Cohort Size Effect In the Time Windows Of 100-20mentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Not all semantic neighbors exert equal effects, as close semantic neighbors will compete to a greater extent than distant semantic neighbors. For example, naming a picture of a dog is frequently slower in the presence of a near semantic neighbor (e.g., cat) than a distant semantic neighbor (e.g., whale) or an unrelated distractor (e.g., couch; see Damian et al, 2001;Fieder et al, 2018;Rose et al, 2018;Vieth et al, 2014; but see Mahon, Costa, Peterson, Vargas, & Caramazza, 2007). The semantic properties that influence neighborhood distance may plausibly include shape, size, color, typical location, or other taxonomic or event-related (thematic) features.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the domains of language and semantic memory, considerable evidence suggests that the sharing of semantic (e.g., visual or propositional) or phonological features influences competition. For example, the word "robin" may interfere with the production of the word "ostrich" by virtue of the sharing of visual and propositional feature "has wings" (e.g., see Collins & Loftus, 1975;Fieder, Wartenburger, & Abdel Rahman, 2018;Mirman & Magnuson, 2008;Rose, Aristei, Melinger, & Abdel Rahman, 2018;Vieth, McMahon, & de Zubicaray, 2014;Vigliocco, Vinson, Lewis, & Garrett, 2004); as representations with shared features are more likely to compete for selection (Damian, Vigliocco, & Levelt, 2001;Luce & Pisoni, 1998;Magnuson, Dixon, Tanenhaus, & Aslin, 2007;Vigliocco et al, 2004; for review, see Chen & Mirman, 2012). Furthermore, research in patients with language deficits demonstrates that competition among neighboring word representations is exacerbated in the presence of lesions to the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), resulting in a greater number of selection-related errors in picture naming tasks (e.g., see Ries, Karzmark, Navarrete, Knight, & Dronkers, 2015;Schnur et al, 2009;Thompson-Schill et al, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%