1990
DOI: 10.2307/854046
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Classic Turn of Phrase: Music and the Psychology of Convention

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Yet few such studies have applied the schema concept to the variety of recurrent harmonic and melodic ending formulae found in Western tonal music-what theorists and composers have for centuries called cadences. Indeed, the tonal cadence is generally considered to be the quintessential phrase-level event schema (Eberlein, 1997;Eberlein & Fricke, 1992;Gjerdingen, 1988;Meyer, 1967;Rosner & Narmour, 1992;Sears, 2015Sears, , 2016Sears, Caplin, & McAdams, 2014;Temperley, 2004), with the specificity of the mental representation reflected in the strength and specificity of the schematic expectations it generates. And yet experimental studies explicitly examining the perception of cadences (or cadential closure) are notably few (e.g., Tillmann, Bigand, & Madurell, 1998;Sears et al, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet few such studies have applied the schema concept to the variety of recurrent harmonic and melodic ending formulae found in Western tonal music-what theorists and composers have for centuries called cadences. Indeed, the tonal cadence is generally considered to be the quintessential phrase-level event schema (Eberlein, 1997;Eberlein & Fricke, 1992;Gjerdingen, 1988;Meyer, 1967;Rosner & Narmour, 1992;Sears, 2015Sears, , 2016Sears, Caplin, & McAdams, 2014;Temperley, 2004), with the specificity of the mental representation reflected in the strength and specificity of the schematic expectations it generates. And yet experimental studies explicitly examining the perception of cadences (or cadential closure) are notably few (e.g., Tillmann, Bigand, & Madurell, 1998;Sears et al, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bigand and Poulin-Charronnat ( 2006) discuss how some music-perceptual abilities result purely from exposure to (experience of) music, whereas others require explicit training. Their study does not include judgement of stylistic success, but we extrapolate to hypothesise that the less experienced our listeners, the less likely they are to notice unidiomatic chords or the absence of appropriate musical schemata (Gjerdingen, 1988(Gjerdingen, , 2007, and so the higher on average their ratings of stylistic success will be compared to more experienced listeners.…”
Section: Music Transformer Will Outperform Musicvae On Stylistic Succ...mentioning
confidence: 88%
“…8 Its outputs have been the subject of multiple, rigorously conducted listening studies (Collins et al, 2016(Collins et al, , 2017, and the starting point for use by artists in the AI Song Contest. 9 Research by Gjerdingen (1988) on the Classical style suggests excerpts up to 4 bars in length can sound stylistically coherent without structural inheritance. When structural inheritance is required by a MAIA Markov user, it is accomplished by hard-coding a repetitive structure (e.g., reuse of bars 1-4 in bars 5-8) or running a pattern discovery algorithm such as SIARCT (Collins et al, 2013(Collins et al, , 2010 to obtain one automatically.…”
Section: Sequential Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It was important that the samples were defined in a way that instances of the schema could not be 'cherry-picked' (as recognised in Meyer 1989, pp. 57-9, andCavett-Dunsby 1990). Entire genres of composers were used to demarcate samples because they comprise predefined collections of works and movements.…”
Section: Harmonic-rhythm Ratiosmentioning
confidence: 99%