2011
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-011-0147-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A bias to detail: how hand position modulates visual learning and visual memory

Abstract: In this report, we examine whether and how altered aspects of perception and attention near the hands affect one's learning of to-be-remembered visual material. We employed the contextual cuing paradigm of visual learning in two experiments. Participants searched for a target embedded within images of fractals and other complex geometrical patterns while either holding their hands near to or far from the stimuli. When visual features and structural patterns remained constant across to-be-learned images (Exp. 1… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
32
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 51 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
(50 reference statements)
2
32
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, detection of contralesional visual stimuli can improve in a patient with a lesion of the visual cortex, when these stimuli are placed within hand reach (Schendel & Robertson, 2004). This finding has now been extended to healthy humans, with advantages in visual processing documented in a range of perceptual (Abrams, Davoli, Du, Knapp, & Paull, 2008;Cosman & Vecera, 2010;Reed, Grubb, & Steele, 2006;Tseng & Bridgeman, 2011) and learning tasks (Davoli, Brockmole, & Goujon, 2012). Furthermore, it has been revealed even when the proximity between the hands and the visual stimuli is only an imagined posture (Davoli & Abrams, 2009).…”
Section: Partially Automatic Orienting For Visual Targetsmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…Similarly, detection of contralesional visual stimuli can improve in a patient with a lesion of the visual cortex, when these stimuli are placed within hand reach (Schendel & Robertson, 2004). This finding has now been extended to healthy humans, with advantages in visual processing documented in a range of perceptual (Abrams, Davoli, Du, Knapp, & Paull, 2008;Cosman & Vecera, 2010;Reed, Grubb, & Steele, 2006;Tseng & Bridgeman, 2011) and learning tasks (Davoli, Brockmole, & Goujon, 2012). Furthermore, it has been revealed even when the proximity between the hands and the visual stimuli is only an imagined posture (Davoli & Abrams, 2009).…”
Section: Partially Automatic Orienting For Visual Targetsmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…To account for the differences in baseline search rates across groups in our analyses of learning, we focused on a proportional measure of performance (a cuing effect) where the difference in search time between the unrepeated and repeated trials was divided by the search performance observed for unrepeated trials (see Brockmole et al, 2008;Davoli, Brockmole, & Goujon, 2012). Using this procedure, for example, a cuing effect of .33 would indicate that search performance for repeated trials was one third faster than unrepeated trials.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As a result, the exposure duration to the displays (usually longer in fMRI studies), the size of the display, the distance to the monitor, the visual angle, and the color of the items are all characteristics that can influence the width of the spatial window of attention, and consequentially whether the context will be encoded at a local or global level. For example, it has been shown that CC within visually-complex images was affected by the proximity with which the hands were held to the stimuli, suggesting a bias toward detail-oriented processing near the hands [85].…”
Section: Mediating Factors In CCmentioning
confidence: 99%