1997
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.23.5.1343
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A belongingness principle of motion perception.

Abstract: Four experiments are reported that investigated the role of the perceived coplanarity of a moving target with respect to a frame of reference in the third dimension on the perceived path of that target. When a target dot and small moving frame appeared coplanar, the dot's perceived trajectory was governed entirely by its changing position relative to the moving frame. However, when the target and a large stationary frame appeared in a different plane than the small moving frame, the motion of the dot was seen … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

3
5
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
3
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Factoring out these biases thus would only cause an overall upward shift, not affect the statistical analysis. The drop in the effect The results replicate the distance-dependent influence of motionbased reference frames on the perceived motion of nearby objects that has already been shown by several studies (DiVita & Rock, 1997;Hochberg & Fallon, 1976;Mori, 1979;Shum & Wolford, 1983). All of these studies lend support to the claim that common motion might serve as a reference frame, but its effectiveness is limited to a spatial region.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 76%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Factoring out these biases thus would only cause an overall upward shift, not affect the statistical analysis. The drop in the effect The results replicate the distance-dependent influence of motionbased reference frames on the perceived motion of nearby objects that has already been shown by several studies (DiVita & Rock, 1997;Hochberg & Fallon, 1976;Mori, 1979;Shum & Wolford, 1983). All of these studies lend support to the claim that common motion might serve as a reference frame, but its effectiveness is limited to a spatial region.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 76%
“…Moreover, as was acknowledged by the authors, their model in its current form cannot account for induced motion, in which a stationary object is seen to be moving in the opposite direction from a surrounding (or neighboring) moving object. This is partly due to a claim that vector decomposition and induced motion arise from different neural mechanisms (DiVita & Rock, 1997). For instance, induced motion is not perceived after the motion threshold (up to 3°/ s) for the frame is reached.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The model would need to be refined to account for induced motion displays using an oscillating rectangle to induce an opposite perceived motion direction in a static dot (Duncker, 1929(Duncker, /1938. The suggestion that additional mechanisms are needed to explain induced motion is supported by experimental evidence highlighting differences between induced motion and vector decomposition, as summarized by Di Vita and Rock (1997). For example, induced motion is typically not observed when the reference frame's physical speed is above the threshold for motion detection, whereas the vector decomposition stimuli analyzed here are robust to variations in speed.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 48%
“…Subtraction of the commonmotion component is due to inhibition from cells coding for the nearer depth to cells coding for the farther depth. We show below how interdepth directional inhibition causes a peak shift (Grossberg & Levine, 1976) in directional selectivity that behaves like a vector decomposition.Following Johansson (1950), vector decomposition has been invoked to explain motion perception in multiple experiments employing a variety of stimulus configurations (e.g., Börjesson & von Hofsten, 1972, 1975, 1977Cutting & Proffitt, 1982;Di Vita & Rock, 1997;Gogel & MacCracken, 1979;Gogel & Tietz, 1976;Johansson, 1974;Post, Chi, Heckmann, & Chaderjian, 1989). The bulk of this work supports the view that vector decomposition is a useful concept in characterizing object-centric frames of reference in motion perception.…”
supporting
confidence: 57%