2000
DOI: 10.1023/a:1007833125015
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Untitled

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast, when R was favorable (i.e., p = .90) they tended to be loss averse, overweighting losses relative to gains (i.e., λ > 1). Follow-up work refined the original methodology (Chechile & Butler, 2000, 2003; Chechile & Luce, 1999) but the take-home message is the same: the subjective representation of outcomes is context-dependent, varying as a function of the favorableness of the reference lottery. Future work should attempt to reconcile these results with Walasek and Stewart’s and the present ones.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, when R was favorable (i.e., p = .90) they tended to be loss averse, overweighting losses relative to gains (i.e., λ > 1). Follow-up work refined the original methodology (Chechile & Butler, 2000, 2003; Chechile & Luce, 1999) but the take-home message is the same: the subjective representation of outcomes is context-dependent, varying as a function of the favorableness of the reference lottery. Future work should attempt to reconcile these results with Walasek and Stewart’s and the present ones.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, when R was favorable (i.e., p = .90) they tended to be loss averse, overweighting losses relative to gains (i.e., λ > 1). Follow-up work refined the original methodology (Chechile & Butler, 2000, 2003Chechile & Luce, 1999) Solid lines depict the fit of simple linear regressions. In both panels, predicted choice proportions below 50% reflect loss aversion, those above 50% gain seeking.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%