2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.040
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Defining the most probable location of the parahippocampal place area using cortex-based alignment and cross-validation

Abstract: The parahippocampal place area (PPA) is a widely studied high-level visual region in the human brain involved in place and scene processing. The goal of the present study was to identify the most probable location of place-selective voxels in medial ventral temporal cortex. To achieve this goal, we first used cortex-based alignment (CBA) to create a probabilistic place-selective region of interest (ROI) from one group of 12 participants. We then tested how well this ROI could predict place selectivity in each … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

4
90
2
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

4
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 75 publications
(97 citation statements)
references
References 89 publications
(139 reference statements)
4
90
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, areas composing the visual processing hierarchy have been identified in living brains using anatomical and functional magnetic resonance imaging [2][3][4][5][6] , as well as in postmortem brains based on cytoarchitecture [7][8][9][10][11][12][13] , myeloarchitecture 14,15 , and receptor architecture [16][17][18] . Interestingly, and contrary to classic findings [19][20][21][22] , recent research in human postmortem brains indicates a tight correspondence between cellular transitions of cytoarchitectonic areas and cortical folding across the visual hierarchy 13,[23][24][25] . Similarly, recent research also indicates a tight correspondence between functional regions and cortical folding across the visual hierarchy for early 26 , middle [27][28][29] , and late 23,24,30 visual regions.…”
Section: Introductioncontrasting
confidence: 82%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, areas composing the visual processing hierarchy have been identified in living brains using anatomical and functional magnetic resonance imaging [2][3][4][5][6] , as well as in postmortem brains based on cytoarchitecture [7][8][9][10][11][12][13] , myeloarchitecture 14,15 , and receptor architecture [16][17][18] . Interestingly, and contrary to classic findings [19][20][21][22] , recent research in human postmortem brains indicates a tight correspondence between cellular transitions of cytoarchitectonic areas and cortical folding across the visual hierarchy 13,[23][24][25] . Similarly, recent research also indicates a tight correspondence between functional regions and cortical folding across the visual hierarchy for early 26 , middle [27][28][29] , and late 23,24,30 visual regions.…”
Section: Introductioncontrasting
confidence: 82%
“…Interestingly, and contrary to classic findings [19][20][21][22] , recent research in human postmortem brains indicates a tight correspondence between cellular transitions of cytoarchitectonic areas and cortical folding across the visual hierarchy 13,[23][24][25] . Similarly, recent research also indicates a tight correspondence between functional regions and cortical folding across the visual hierarchy for early 26 , middle [27][28][29] , and late 23,24,30 visual regions. In terms of the latter, this striking consistency is conserved across development [30][31][32][33][34] , and is causally implicated in different aspects of visual perception [35][36][37][38][39] .…”
Section: Introductioncontrasting
confidence: 82%
“…The interplay between brain structure and function in concerting human cognition has spiked considerable interest in recent years. One of the most fascinating observations in this context is the consistent spatial arrangement of category-selective regions in ventral temporal cortex (VTC) across individuals Grill-Spector and Weiner, 2014;Kanwisher, 2010;Weiner et al, 2018;Weiner and Grill-Spector, 2010). VTC contains several visual category-selective regions that respond more strongly to their preferred category than others, including regions that are selective to faces (Kanwisher et al, 1997), scenes (Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998), bodies (Peelen and Downing, 2005), words (Cohen et al, 2000) and possibly numbers (Grotheer et al, 2016b(Grotheer et al, , 2016aShum et al, 2013, but see Grotheer et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this approach, participants perform a short task in addition to the main task in the scanning session, and the data from this task is used to localize regions-of-interest to be tested with data from the main task. This method is commonly used in vision research (Erez & Yovel, 2014;Lafer-Sousa, Conway, & Kanwisher, 2016;Reddy & Kanwisher, 2007;Weiner et al, 2018). For example, specific tasks are used to identify regions in individuals that are recruited for face processing (Berman et al, 2010;Kanwisher, McDermott, & Chun, 1997) and object processing (Malach et al, 1995).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%