2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.08.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Explicit and implicit approach vs. avoidance tendencies towards high vs. low calorie food cues in patients with anorexia nervosa and healthy controls

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

2
48
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

5
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 56 publications
(51 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
2
48
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…More tentatively, both relevant feature and irrelevant feature versions of the task show some criterion validity in that AAT biases relate to higher state craving. Together, these results have the potential to open new avenues for applications of the AAT to relevant populations thought to be characterized by increased appetitive responding to food cues (e.g., food craving; Brockmeyer et al, 2015b), reduced appetitive responding (e.g., anorexia nervosa; Neimeijer, de Jong, & Roefs, 2015;Paslakis et al, 2016) or ambivalent responding (Hormes & Rozin, 2011; Rodríguez, Fernández, Cepeda-Benito, & Vila, 2005). It also poses crucial questions with regard to how AAT training protocols should be designed (Becker, Jostmann, Wiers, & Holland, 2015;Brockmeyer et al, 2015a;Kakoschke, Kemps, & Tiggemann, 2017;Wiers et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…More tentatively, both relevant feature and irrelevant feature versions of the task show some criterion validity in that AAT biases relate to higher state craving. Together, these results have the potential to open new avenues for applications of the AAT to relevant populations thought to be characterized by increased appetitive responding to food cues (e.g., food craving; Brockmeyer et al, 2015b), reduced appetitive responding (e.g., anorexia nervosa; Neimeijer, de Jong, & Roefs, 2015;Paslakis et al, 2016) or ambivalent responding (Hormes & Rozin, 2011; Rodríguez, Fernández, Cepeda-Benito, & Vila, 2005). It also poses crucial questions with regard to how AAT training protocols should be designed (Becker, Jostmann, Wiers, & Holland, 2015;Brockmeyer et al, 2015a;Kakoschke, Kemps, & Tiggemann, 2017;Wiers et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the context of anxiety and addiction research it has been shown that positively-connoted cues were associated with an approach, and negative cues with an avoidance bias [ 32 ]. In a previous investigation, our group has shown that healthy controls displayed an approach bias for high- and low-calorie food cues; this bias was absent in the group of anorexia nervosa (AN) patients [ 33 ]. In a non-clinical cohort, Brockmeyer et al [ 34 ] applied a food approach-avoidance task and could show that high food cravers displayed a stronger automatic approach bias towards food than low food cravers.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although research is limited, one study found that individuals with BN did not differ from controls in consumption after food cue exposure (Nederkoorn, Smulders, Havermans, & Jansen, 2004), although another study found that people with BN report greater urge to binge and lower confidence to resist bingeing than controls (Staiger et al, 2000). In contrast, samples with AN tend to show avoidance biases to palatable food (Paslakis et al, 2016). However, it is unclear if these affective responses to palatable food cues translate to subsequent increased consumption after food cue exposure in experimental paradigms with ED groups.…”
Section: Introduction and Aimsmentioning
confidence: 99%