2016
DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2016.1188890
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cognitive profiles of schizotypal dimensions in a community cohort: Common properties of differential manifestations

Abstract: Our findings further support the more analytical four-factor categorization of schizotypy and suggest that the discrepancies in the findings so far might be due to a more "generalized" definition of the schizotypal dimensions. They also add further in the early formulation of the profile of the high-schizotypal individuals seeking psychiatric help so that their overall management is directed towards a more targeted approach.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
18
0
2

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 102 publications
1
18
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This could also imply that there are differential effects of trauma types on neurocognition, which could not be explored in this study. Nevertheless, processing speed has been proposed as a core cognitive impairment in individuals with schizophrenia and high schizotypy , so perhaps it is not surprising that the deficits are evident across the SPD population and could not be explained by childhood traumatic experiences alone. On the other hand, it is possible that group differences between high and low/no trauma reporters were not observed due to a floor effect in TMT‐A scoring (time to complete the task).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This could also imply that there are differential effects of trauma types on neurocognition, which could not be explored in this study. Nevertheless, processing speed has been proposed as a core cognitive impairment in individuals with schizophrenia and high schizotypy , so perhaps it is not surprising that the deficits are evident across the SPD population and could not be explained by childhood traumatic experiences alone. On the other hand, it is possible that group differences between high and low/no trauma reporters were not observed due to a floor effect in TMT‐A scoring (time to complete the task).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Working memory deficits are less consistently reported in those in the at‐risk mental state (Bang et al, ; Carrion et al, ). Associations have also been explored with the phenotypic related trait of schizotypy: Results are inconsistent with some studies reporting reduced working memory performance (Lui et al, ; Matheson & Langdon, ) whereas others do not (Karagiannopoulou et al, ; Noguchi, Hori, & Kunugi, ). One study suggested stress levels might account for the reduced working memory performance in patients with schizophrenia when compared to relatives and healthy individuals (Krkovic, Moritz, & Lincoln, ).…”
Section: Psychotic Disorders and Working Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3,[12][13][14][15] Evidence from both correlational (making full use of variance in schizotypy scores) and categorical (allocating individuals to discrete groups on the basis of schizotypy scores) studies suggests subtle yet fairly widespread cognitive alterations in high schizotypy, 6 including poorer performance in tasks measuring cognitive control. [16][17][18][19][20][21][22][23][24][25] Cognitive control, or executive function, involves the domain-independent representation and maintenance of task goals, thereby supporting the flexible adaptation of information processing and behavior in a changing environment. Importantly, cognitive control refers to a heterogeneous set of functions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%