2014
DOI: 10.1177/0956797614531024
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Justifying Atrocities

Abstract: A burgeoning literature has established that exposure to atrocities committed by in-group members triggers moral-disengagement strategies. There is little research, however, on how such moral disengagement affects the degree to which conversations shape people's memories of the atrocities and subsequent justifications for those atrocities. We built on the finding that a speaker's selective recounting of past events can result in retrieval-induced forgetting of related, unretrieved memories for both the speaker… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0
2

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

5
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
0
14
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…We should caution that SSRIF is not inevitable when group membership is both shared and salient. In a study aimed at exploring the mechanisms by which moral disengagement strategies affect social remembering, Coman, Stone, Castano, and Hirst (2014) examined American listeners attending to American speakers' accounts of atrocities in which previously studied justifications went unmentioned. They found SSRIF for the justifications when the perpetrators of the atrocities were Iraqis.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We should caution that SSRIF is not inevitable when group membership is both shared and salient. In a study aimed at exploring the mechanisms by which moral disengagement strategies affect social remembering, Coman, Stone, Castano, and Hirst (2014) examined American listeners attending to American speakers' accounts of atrocities in which previously studied justifications went unmentioned. They found SSRIF for the justifications when the perpetrators of the atrocities were Iraqis.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The former process is known as within-individual retrieval-induced forgetting (WIRIF), and the latter as socially shared retrieval-induced forgetting (SSRIF, Cuc, Koppel, & Hirst, 2007;Stone, Barnier, Sutton, & Hirst, 2010, 2013. Built off of the retrieval-induced forgetting paradigm (RIF; Anderson, Bjork, & Bjork, 1994), researchers examining SSRIF have found such forgetting for a number of different stimuli, including stories (Cuc et al, 2007;Stone et al, 2010), autobiographical memories (Stone et al, 2013), flashbulb memories (Coman, Manier, & Hirst, 2009); academic material (Koppel et al, 2014), medical information (Coman, Coman, & Hirst, 2013), beliefs (Vlasceanu & Coman, 2018), atrocities (Coman, Stone, Castano, & Hirst, 2014), and controversial topics (i.e., euthanasia; . As this research makes clear, SSRIF is a robust phenomenon.…”
Section: Socially Shared Retrieval-induced Forgettingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This desire for greater social distance toward groups deemed different from oneself may additionally be nourished by the general tendency to perceive one's outgroup as more homogeneous and one's ingroup as more heterogeneous (Mullen & Hu, 1989), which may also occur in personal narratives. Indeed, personal historical memories have been found to be biased in favor of one's ingroup and against other groups (Blatz & Ross, 2009;Coman, Stone, Castano, & Hirst, 2014;Welzer et al, 2002). This ingroup preference may further be influenced by the salience of one's own and others' group identity when reminiscing about the shared past (Coman & Hirst, 2015).…”
Section: Narrative Meaning Making and Social Cognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%